East Tennessee State University East Tennessee State University
Digital Commons @ East Digital Commons @ East
Tennessee State University Tennessee State University
Electronic Theses and Dissertations Student Works
12-2018
Use of a Daily Behavior Report Card and Response Dependent Use of a Daily Behavior Report Card and Response Dependent
Fading to Increase and Maintain Academic Engagement in an Fading to Increase and Maintain Academic Engagement in an
Elementary Student with Developmental Delay Elementary Student with Developmental Delay
Ziyad Alrumayh
East Tennessee State University
Follow this and additional works at: https://dc.etsu.edu/etd
Part of the Special Education and Teaching Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Alrumayh, Ziyad, "Use of a Daily Behavior Report Card and Response Dependent Fading to Increase and
Maintain Academic Engagement in an Elementary Student with Developmental Delay" (2018).
Electronic
Theses and Dissertations.
Paper 3514. https://dc.etsu.edu/etd/3514
This Thesis - unrestricted is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Works at Digital Commons @
East Tennessee State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an
authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University. For more information, please
Use of a Daily Behavior Report Card and Response Dependent Fading to Increase and Maintain
Academic Engagement in an Elementary Student with Developmental Delay
A thesis
presented to
the faculty of the Department of Special Education
In partial fulfillment
of the requirements for the degree
Master of Arts in Special Education
by
Ziyad Alrumayh
December 2018
Dr. James Fox, Chair
Dr. Tina Hudson
Dr. Sarah Hitt
Key Words: Daily Behavior Report Card, Response Dependent Fading System, Academic
Engagement, behavior challenges, and ADHD
2
ABSTRACT
Use of a Daily Behavior Report Card and Response Dependent Fading to Increase and Maintain
Academic Engagement in an Elementary Student with Developmental Delay
by
Ziyad Alrumayh
The initial and maintenance effects of a Daily Behavior Report Card (DBRC) intervention and
fading procedure on Academic Engaged Time (AET) of a 6-year-old boy with Developmental
Delay and Attention Deficits were evaluated with a reversal design. Following an initial
baseline, the DBRC was implemented and then completely removed followed by reapplication
of the DBRC. Subsequently, the report card was gradually reduced in the frequency of its use
from very daily to every other day. Increased AET reliably varied with the application and then
removal of the DBRC. Whereas complete removal of the DBRC resulted in substantial
decreases in AET, response dependent fading of the card was associated with AET of 80% or
above. Teacher intervention ratings demonstrated social validity of the intervention in terms of
its effectiveness, acceptability and efficiency. Research and practical issues are discussed.
3
DEDICATION
This thesis is dedicated to my mother, Hessa Alblehed, father, Saleh Alrumayh, to my
entire family, and my friend Khaled Almuaigl for providing the necessary support required
throughout the learning process. Through their support and unconditional love, they have set a
good example towards enlightening me on how to work hard for the things I aspire to achieve.
Finally, I would also like to thank my colleagues and friends whose names have not been
mentioned here due to paucity of space but have in, various ways, contributed to the thesis. I
would like to reiterate that their kindness and commitment to my academic journey are
invaluable and I will forever be indebted to you.
4
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I offer my sincerest gratitude to my advisor, Dr. Fox, who has guided and supported me
throughout this thesis with his patience and knowledge whilst allowing me the opportunity to
work in my way. I attribute the level of my master’s degree to his encouragement and effort as,
without him, this thesis would not have been completed. To my committee Dr. Tina and Dr.
Hitt, this work would not have been a success without your immense help and valuable
guidance throughout the research process. To that, I am immensely grateful.
5
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page
ABSTRACT……………………………………………………………………………....…….2
DEDICATION…….………………………………………………………………………….3
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS……………………………..…………………………………...….4
LIST OF TABLEST….………………………………………………………………………....7
LIST OF FIGURES……………...…………………………………………………………...…8
Chapter
1. INTRODUCTION…………..……………..…………………………………………..……..9
Developmental Delay……………………………….………………………......……....9
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)…………………..……..………....10
Behavior challenges……………………………………......…………………..……...15
Positive behavior support…………………………………………...………………...16
Interventions…………………………………………………………………………...17
Objectives of this study…………………………………………………………..……19
2. LITERATURE REVIEW ……………………………………………………………….….21
Daily Behavior Report Cards………………………….…………………….............…21
Response Dependent Fading System….……………………………….……………....27
3. METHODOLOGY………………………………………………………………...………..29
Participant….……………………..…………………….……………………………...29
Study Setting……………………………………………………………………….......30
Materials….………………………………………..………………….……………….30
Measures….………………..……………….…………….……………………………………31
6
Dependent variable.………………..…………………….………………………….31
Interobserver Agreement………………………………………………….………...…32
Functional Analysis Screening Tool……………………………………..………...….33
Social Validity….………………………………………..……………………….…....34
Procedures………………………………………………………………………………...……35
Intervention….………………………….………………………….………………..…35
Experimental Design …………………………………………………………….……37
4. RESULTS…………………………………………………………………………………...38
IOA……………………………………………………………………………………38
AET…………………………………………………………………………………….39
DBRC…………………………………………………………………………...……..40
Social Validity…………………………………………………………………...…….42
5. DISCUSSION……………………………………………….………………………………44
DBRC ………………………………..……………………………………..…………44
Limitations & Future Research…………………………………………………...…....50
REFERENCES …………………………………….………………………………..……..….53
APPENDIXS……………………………………………………………………………...…..60
Appendix A Daily Behavior Report Card…………………………………………………..…60
Appendix B Academic Engaged Time Definition and Observation………………………......61
Appendix C Functional Assessment Screening Tool/FAST………………………………......64
Appendix D Intervention Rating Profile – Teacher………………………………………........66
Appendix E Institutional Review Board Approval Letter…………………………………..…67
VITA………………………………………………………………………...…………………68
7
LIST OF TABLES
Table Page
1. Interobserver Agreement per Session………………………………………………………..…38
2. Academic Engaged Time: Change in Mean Level and Percentage of Overlap…………….….40
8
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure Page
1. Daily Percentage of and Trend in Bobby’s Academic Engaged Time per Phase……..……….39
2. The Percentage of DBRC Behaviors for Which Bobby Received Points…………………..….41
9
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
Developmental Delay
Developmental delay is a condition when a child exhibits substantial lag in achieving
certain cognitive or physical milestones which become increasingly noticeable over the initial
months of infancy. Children do not have any fixed time-table to develop the skills. It is normal
to expect some variation in their development as each child differs from another (Dyck & Piek,
2014). And at times, the developmental delays are not usually the cause of major concern as the
differences are marginal and get made-up with time. However, it emerges as a more serious
issue when a child is consistently behind in basic skills like walking, reading or speaking in
comparison to his peers (Koul, Al-Yahmedy, & Al-Futaisi, 2012).).
At times, it is observed that the terms developmental disabilities and developmental
delay are used interchangeably. But they have distinct meaning, developmental disabilities are
such issues that children do not outgrow or catch up from, though it is still possible to make
some progress with proper interventions and medications. Developmental disabilities are often
results of underlying problems like Down Syndrome, autism, fetal alcohol spectrum disorders
(FASD) and brain injuries. On the other hand of the spectrum, developmental delays can be the
initial symptoms of any underlying learning or attention issues (Dyck & Piek, 2014).
Developmental delay can occur in five major areas of development. The areas are
cognitive, social and emotional skills; speech and language skills; fine and gross motor skills;
and activities undertaken in day to day lives.
Several studies have shed light on the link between developmental delays and learning
and attention issues, it has been found that such delays could be the very first sign of learning
10
and attention issues (Dyck & Pike, 2014 ; Koul et al., 2012). For instance, if there is a child who
is showing signs of speech and language delays can well insinuate that learning or
communication disorder could be the underlying problem.
Furthermore, it has been assessed that it is not always feasible to establish the link
between development delays and attention disorders until the students start their school and
academic life. It is so because, in the learner setting, students become increasingly exposed to
studying math, science, reading and writing that make their developmental delays more
noticeable (Dyck & Piek, 2014). If a child shows the signs of developmental delay, the first
round of intervention comprises of functional test assessment and if it is deemed fit, the child is
enrolled into the special education program. In many of such cases, early detection that is
followed by early intervention is shown to have a better prognosis than those who do not
receive any such treatment for a delayed period of time (Dyck & Pike, 2014 ; Koul et al., 2012).
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
ADHD is a complex neurological mental disorder that is characterized by a student
having problems paying attention, absent-mindedness, abnormal level of energy or excessive
impulsivity which is often not age appropriate.
The student is also excessively active and is unable to control his/her behavior which is
inappropriate for his/her age. The symptoms begin appearing when a child is below 12 years
old. The condition causes problems in three main settings: the school, the home and at
recreational facilities. For the children who have the problem of paying attention, it may hamper
their performance at school. Though the condition causes impairments, the majority of the
children have an attention span that can enable them to perform some tasks. Irrespective of the
numerous studies that have been conducted; the exact cause of the condition in most of the
11
cases is unknown. It has been discovered that using the DSM-IV basis, it is estimated that about
5 to 7 percent of elementary school children have been affected in America. However, if the rate
of prevalence is assessed through ICD-10, the percentage drops by 1 to 2 percent. By 2015, the
figures showed that it affected 51.1 million people in America (Fabiano & Pelham, 2003).
These rates are similar in different countries, however; it also depends on how the
diagnosis is done. Elementary students with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
exhibit significant social, behavioral, and academic challenges in academic settings. Children
with ADHD have difficulty in sustaining attention for long and often end up exhibiting
significant levels of hyperactivity-impulsivity (DuPaul & Stoner, 2003). Issues with ADHD
may not exhibit in home settings as they do in classroom settings because of the nature of a
classroom where learners are expected to be organized, sustain attention, maintain good
behavior, listen attentively, and complete assignments on time. Consequently, ADHD
elementary students face challenges that hinder them from fulfilling the requirements necessary
for appropriate achievement in their respective class settings. Symptoms exhibited by learners
with ADHD impact the affected learners, their classmates, and the instructors who face
instruction difficulties because of impulsivity, hyperactivity, and inattention.
According to Fabiano and Pelham (2003), ADHD is a problem affecting about 3% to
5% of elementary school-attending population. On average, these percentages place at least one
elementary learner with ADHD in every classroom in the U.S. According to the Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), the numbers of children with ADHD continued to
increase from 7.8% in 2003 to 11% in 2012. Because of the identified challenges and the
prevalence rates, learning institutions with students battling with ADHD choose to implement
effective interventions for minimizing the classroom impairment characteristics of students with
12
ADHD. Such interventions include the use of the Daily Behavior Report Card for the student
with ADHD (DuPaul & Stoner, 2003).
Based on the DSM-5 diagnosis, the symptoms in a child must be present for a minimum
of six months to a level that is higher than that of the other children of the same age for it to be
classified as ADHD. They must also create significant problems in at least two main settings:
home, school, work or social. This full criterion must be met before the child attains the age of
12.ADHD is of three categories: those who are predominantly attentive (ADHD-P1 or ADHD-
1), the predominantly hyperactive-impulsive (ADHD-PH or ADHD-HI) and the ones who have
a combination of ADHD-C.A child who has the inattentive type of ADHD displays all the
following characteristics except in situations where there is an explanation by a psychiatric or a
doctor (DuPaul & Stoner, 2004).
They are easily distracted, forget things easily, miss details and they frequently change
from one activity to another and unable to sustain a focus on one task. They get easily bored
with one task after a few minutes and can sustain their interest a little bit longer if the task is
enjoyable. They do not focus their attention on completing or organizing a task. They find it
difficult working on homework and sometimes do not complete them. They easily lose things
such as pens, pencils, toys, and books (Dyck & Pike, 2014 ; Koul et al., 2012). When they are
spoken to, they feign nonchalance as if they are not listening. Because they often indulge in
daydreaming, they get easily confused and they, therefore, move very slowly. It is very difficult
for them to easily process information and accurately as the rest of the students. They struggle
and find it hard to follow instructions. They find it difficult to understand details they,
therefore, overlook them (DuPaul & Stoner, 2003).
13
The children who have ADHD hyperactive-impulse exhibit characteristics such as they
squirm, talk and fidget excessively. They move around touching and playing with anything they
lay their hands on and are unable to stay still when having meals, doing homework and even
during story time. They are constantly moving, impatient and find it hard to perform tasks that
require quietness. They show their emotions without any form of restraint. They do not mind
the consequences of their actions and find it difficult waiting for the things that they want. They
are unable to wait for their turns in games. They frequently interrupt other childrens activities
or conversations and are unable to form and sustain friendship. The girls are less affected by
hyperactivity attention in comparison to boys but show greater symptoms regarding
distractibility (Loe & Feldman, 2007).
The Disruptive Behavior Disorder (ODD) is two dimensional because it includes two
disorders that are similar: the Opposition Defiant Disorder and (ODD) and the Conduct
Disorder (CD).The children who have these disorders exhibit the following symptoms: They
defy the authority including the parents, they show angry outbursts and display other behaviors
that are defiant, they start lying and even stealing. The main difference between the conduct
disorder and oppositional defiant disorder lies in the severity of the symptoms exhibited as well
as on a continuum that is based on the progression of development from ODD to CD as the
child grows (Loe & Feldman, 2007).
ODD means a pattern of negative defiant disobedience that is recurrent. It also shows a
behavior that is hostile towards the figures of authority which last for a period of six months.
This group of children loses tempers easily, argues with authority/adults, refuses to follow the
directions of the adult's commands and requests, annoys people deliberately, blames others for
the mistakes that are not theirs, resentful and angry, quick and easy to annoy other people.CD
14
involves behaviors that are more severe which includes developing aggression towards people
and animals, careless destruction of property, skipping school, lying and even stealing. The
behaviors that are connected with CD are commonly referred to as delinquent (Volpe et al.,
2006).
Poor reading and mathematical abilities, poor grades, and increased grade retention are
associated with ADHD among elementary school learners. Learners who show inattention,
hyperactivity, and impulsive symptoms associated with ADHD exhibit poor academic and
educational outcomes. The disorder affects their learning abilities through impacting higher-
level cognition, problem-solving abilities, time management, and judgment. Accordingly,
ADHD leads to limitations in learning and knowledge application, including calculation,
reading, and writing. Loe and Feldman (2007) argue that learners with ADHD have issues with
academic performance, which includes assignment completion, and academic
underachievement that denotes problems with knowledge acquisition and use, and the
consequent low grades and test scores. A study by Volpe et al. (2006) reports that students with
ADHD show significant poor academic performance and academic underachievement among
other educational challenges. Besides, they score lower on arithmetic and reading assessments
than students without ADHD (Biederman et al., 1999). Accordingly, these learners are likely to
show the increase in repeat grades, assignment for remedial pullout services, after-school
programs, and placement in special learning institutions. However, the learning abilities and
challenges faced vary depending on the form of ADHD a child has. Loe and Feldman (2007)
argue that studies provide varying data concerning the variation of academic and educational
characteristics of ADHD-I (inattentive) and ADHD-C (combined) because they find no
significant differences in the outcome of academic attainment among the different ADHD
15
students. However, a great number of elementary school students with ADHD-I are more likely
to be rated as below average in academic performance and attainment compared with the
learners with ADHD-C (Rapport, Scanlan, & Denney, 1999). The study’s outcome indicates
that students with ADHD-I have a higher prevalence of learning challenges those with ADHD-
C.
Behavior Challenges
Besides the academic achievement impacts of ADHD and developmental delay among
learners, the condition poses significant behavioral challenges that affect not only the student
but also peers and the teachers. As noted earlier ADHD learners are disruptive, disorganized,
and inattentive whereas students’ experiencing developmental delays have trouble reading,
writing and understanding properly. Consequently, they tend to pay little attention in the
classroom and home settings. Greene et al. (2002) asserts that they also exhibit peer-related
issues such as being overly intrusive and engaging in negative peer interactions that exacerbate
to lack of self-control, argumentativeness with both teachers and fellow students and
aggression. Accordingly, these learners have higher chances of being put in detention,
suspended, expelled, or compelled to repeat a particular grade.
The common behaviors of interrupting conversation and activities and impatience affect
other members in a class. Learners without ADHD may perceive their counterparts as intrusive,
aggressive, and selfish, which affects interpersonal associations and operating in collaborative
learning. Since the ADHD learners have difficulties in waiting for their turn during indoor and
outdoor activities, their counterparts develop enmity, especially when they appear aggressive
(Greene et al., 2002).
16
The behavioral challenges also affect the teachers because they disrupt normal
classroom arrangements, work schedule, learning and assessment sessions, and make time
management difficult. Besides, the challenges task the instructors with more duties of
monitoring the learners to ensure they adhere to classroom rules. The need to treat and manage
learners with ADHD is this crucial among elementary school going children to minimize cases
of impairment and poor academic and learning engagement and achievement. Since children
with ADHD are at greater risk for developing interpersonal and educational issues, it is
important to assess and implement intervention strategies that minimize the rate of academic
failure, early school dropout rates, and juvenile delinquency (Greene et al., 2002).
Positive Behavior Support
Positive behavior support entails engaging in practices that increase positive activities.
Practices that reinforce positive behavior among ADHD students include posting clear rules that
inform the learners what is expected of them in classroom setups or during activities that require
the certain way of operation. The practice further involves reviewing the rules frequently as a
reinforcement method. A different form of positive behavior support involves giving the
learners choices throughout the day. Provision of choice provides them with some sense of
control, which is significant for learners with challenging behaviors. When these learners feel
more in control, they are likely to defy orders because they feel that their opinion matters.
Moreover, it is important to allow them to earn time to participate in their preferred activities as
a way of reinforcing positive behavior such as completing their assignments on time effectively
and following classroom rules. Other positive reinforcement measures include practicing
students for following rules and participating in classroom activities. The positive reinforcement
17
not only reinforces set rules but also increases the student's self-esteem and motivates them to
behave in a certain accepted way (Greene et al., 2002).
Specific administration methodologies can be utilized to encourage scholastic
engagement and lessen unmistakable behavioral side effects of ADHD. This segment focuses
on both precursor centered and outcome arranged methodologies that educators may use to
oversee the progress of scholastic engagement and overall impact and feasibility of
administration methodologies.
Interventions
Students who start showing signs of developmental delays and it is hinted that they
might be suffering from ADHD further have the tendency to react empathically towards the
administered methods that are employed to help them perform well academically and reduce the
incidences of target behaviors that are typically associated with students suffering from
developmental delays and ADHD (Fox et al., 1986). The various methodologies that are
employed to assist the target students are token currencies, reaction cost methodology and
behavioral contracts. Such methodologies are employed with an intention to reduce the
incidences of target problem behaviors that play a detrimental role in exacerbating the child’s
academic performances.
In the past, teachers and educators have readily employed instructional approach such as
repeatedly educating the child a particular social ability, teaching the right ways to ask for help
or request administrating and monitoring their behaviors, incorporating positive and negative
reinforcements as a response to their target behaviors.
For effective intervention it is imperative that educators recall and record the outbursts
of behavior or any target behavior that reduces the academic engagements in the school setting.
18
Students who have developmental delays or exhibit signs of ADHD need more scrutinized
attention and focus than the rest of the peers. Teachers often have to treat such students in a
customized and highly individualized fashion as the regular teaching methodologies might not
work well with needs and requirements of such students.
The typical token intervention framework comprises of certain predetermined class
objectives to be met (for instance, a culmination of autonomous seat-work) and the kind of
auxiliary reinforce (token) to be utilized (for instance, poker chips, checks marks) is chosen.
Furthermore, it is decided on what number of tokens is to be earned by showing target practices
(educators can separate more intricate undertakings into segment parts and give tokens for each
part). Eventually, it is shared with the students the types of reward they can achieve in exchange
of such tokens, (for instance, five tokens rises to five minutes leisure time at the PC). Thus, the
token system has emerged to be one of the most sought-after intervention strategies for students
who experience the sings of developmental delay or ADHD. At the same time, it is imperative
to monitor the adequacy of the intervention practices in order to decide if they are suitable with
the target practices or not (Piffner, 2011).
Another popular intervention methodology to assist students with developmental
disabilities is behavioral contract. In behavioral contract, the particular or target practices are
illustrated (for instance, number of math questions finished precisely amid situate work task).
Obviously, it is critical that the objective conduct that is sketched out is achievable for the target
student. The intervention practice should oversee that the work that the target student is
expected to complete is in moderation so that it does not become too bothersome for them. It is
important to supervise the level and quanity of work as students with developmental dealy as
well as ADHD have lower instructional level in comparison to their peers. Thus for for setting
19
up a behavioral contract the points that must be borne in mind are: The instructor and the target
student recognize the objective behavior(s) of the agreement. The program is started with an
emphasis on a restricted (for instance, two or less) number of target practices. The practices
ought to be particular, discernible, and emphatically expressed (Gilbertson, 2007). The
objective of the intervention practice is to have the target student achieve a predetermined
objective on a daily basis. For instance, toward the start of utilizing a behavioral contract for
math, the student may choose a movement to remunerate if he/she finishes half of the appointed
problems (Gilbertson, 2007). This is followed by giving support either toward the finish of the
class or by the end of the day. The basis to meet objectives is then gradually expanded as the
target student can meet each new objective level.
The target student can well be incorporated in devising the outline for the program on
matters like what topics the student would like to study first. The students’ inclinations should
also be given due importance as it would motivate and encourage him more to be obident
towards the employed intervention strategies. The students may have favored exercises that he
or she might want to use as prizes for effectively meeting the goal(s). Action prizes may
include: extra time on the PC tending to a creature choosing a book to peruse with the instructor
drawing playing a diversion (Piffner, 2011).
Objectives of This Study
To determine the effectiveness over time of DBRC in increasing the task engagement of
students who have difficulty attending to and engaging in classroom academic activities.
20
To ascertain the extent to which the use of DBRC can be gradually decreased over time
using response dependent fading while maintaining student improved task engagement
80% of the time.
To determine the possible relationship between the function of the student's challenging
off-task behaviors (as indicated by the Functional Assessment Screening Tool) and the
initial effectiveness of the DBRC and fading of the intervention over time.
To determine the extent to which the teacher and the target student(s) view the social
validity of the DBRC as indicated by the adult and child versions of the Intervention
Rating Profile
21
CHAPTER 2
LITERATURE REVIEW
Daily Behavior Report Cards
The daily behavior report cards are behavior modification intervention tools used to
detect and correct inappropriate behavior exhibited by learners, especially those with ADHD
disorders. The daily behavior report card is often used to improve learners’ behavior depending
on feedback from the learner’s instructor. These cards serve as a channel for communicating
with students and their teacher about behavior, especially among children exhibiting
externalizing and disruptive behavioral issues (Volpe & Fabiano, 2013). Although different
learning institutions may have varied forms of the daily behavior report card, it is common for
the cards to have a list of behaviors that have been deemed appropriate objectives for
intervention and strategies for rating the target behavior (Volpe & Fabiano, 2013). Accordingly,
the report cards rate behaviors in terms of frequency and duration. However, the essential
components of the report cards include the frequent feedback that the teacher gives to the
learner and the parent, as well as feedback regarding progress towards behavioral objectives,
and home-based rewards contingent on the child's performance. Moreover, a learning institution
or the teacher implementing the report card behavioral intervention method may choose
between the traditional daily report cards and an electronic daily behavior report card that has
been credited for being effective in increasing parent-teacher communication and reducing
disruptive classroom behaviors (Williams et al., 2012).
It has been recommended that DBRC might be plausible, satisfactory, viable in
advancing a positive understudy, and an approach to expand parent/educator correspondence.
22
Likewise, DBRCs are quite beneficial as they involve wide array of observing and mediation
potential outcomes helpful in dealing with ADHD.
Numerous researchers have been conducted on the role of DBRC in obtaining the
necessary information to guide the development of solutions. A study by Owens et al. (2012)
sought to establish the effectiveness of the daily reporting card (DRC) in influencing school
students to achieve an improvement in their general classroom education. Through including 66
school children with ADHD or other disruptive behavior, the researchers established that 72%
of the sample had a significant improvement, with 8 percent reported a decline. Furthermore,
the study proved that about 78 percent of the children achieved the improvement within the first
month. As such, the study showed a significant effect of DBRC in adjusting the learning
capabilities of students with learning defects. The findings of the study were collaborated by
Vannest et al. (2010) who established that the use of DBRC caused an average improvement of
68 percent in 17 studies used in their meta-analysis. Further, the researchers proved varying, but
significant effects of the student's age, behavior, the breadth of intervention use, reliability
measure, scale construction, and home or school intervention as moderators of the student's
response to intervention. As such, it would be necessary to determine the levels of moderation
of these factors to collaborate the previous studies conducted on the same. Mainly, the use of
DBRC has a significant effect on the correction of learning problems in children with ADHD
(Williams et al., 2012).
This method has been appeared to be exceptionally compelling in molding conduct. In a
landmark research supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, the kids who
consolidated the daily behavior report card technique were appeared to have preferable
controlled ADHD manifestations over the individuals who simply endured the solution alone.
23
For a few kids, the everyday report card procedure alone is adequate to deliver noteworthy
change. Others may require extra methodologies to help control the ADHD side effects that
hinder learning (Additude Editors, 2014). These include: outlining an understudy/educator
objectives and rewards or utilizing a token framework in which a kid gain focuses that can be
exchanged for rewards. Each child is unique and special care must be taken to tweak the system
to his or her needs.
The ability of the teacher or another instructor to obtain accurate information from
observation is critical to the success of the intervention method. Implicatively, it will be
necessary to ensure that the teachers involved, including in this study, are adequately prepared
to obtain the right set of data. Label, Kilgus, and Briesch's (2008) study was categorical in that
teachers achieved a three-level ability to rate student's behavior; specifically, none, in the direct,
and secondary levels. However, the teachers showed a similar ability to identify child behavior
from observation in the three spheres. The findings give freedom to the application of the
method of data collection to include using direct or indirect levels.
Further, the ability to attain interaction with and obtain feedback from children with
learning deficiency is paramount in the exercise. The teachers need to cultivate the correct
environment to foster communication. A study by Fox et al. (1986) involved the extermination
of specific responses and obtained from three children with disabilities from their teacher's
probing. The research indicated that the children responded the teacher's praises and prompted
by attaining a higher frequency of initiated interactions in the classroom setting. Conversely, an
abrupt withdrawal of the teacher's prompting resulted in a similarly sudden reduction in the
children's initiations to interactions (Chafouleas, 2002).
24
The study also established that the observed reactions applied to a response dependent
fading approach of intervention. In this case, the preparation of the teachers in collecting the
data should be guided by this study. They should encourage responses from the children and
consider the level of prompting that accompanied specific reactions while obtaining behavioral
data from the children (Williams et al., 2012).
Burke and Vannest (2008) presented an overview of a web-based electronic system for
monitoring the behavioral progress. It is imperative to monitor behavioral-progress as it helps in
evaluating the responsiveness to various behavioral interventions employed. It further helps in
gauging the positive implications of behavior support introduced in various settings, and the
accomplishment of individualized education program goals and objectives. In this study, the
authors provide a conceptual overview for a criterion-referenced behavioral-progress
monitoring program that is referred to as electronic daily behavioral-progress report card (e-
DBRC) system.
It is extremely pivotal to monitor behavior as it is one of the most critical parts of special
education for students who are dealing with symptoms of ADHD. Monitoring the behavior
helps in gauging how well students are responding to the employed intervention and techniques.
Furthermore, it also helps in assessing how far improvements have been made in achieving the
individualized goals and objectives. The most common approaches for monitoring the progress
made in behavior are by employing the social and behavioral scales. However, it has been found
that many of such social and behavioral skills are too generic and global thus, it is not extremely
effective in properly monitoring the student behavior. A study conducted by Hosp and
colleagues (2003) performed a meta-study of behavior-rating scales and categorized the scales
on the basis of positive action, negative action or absence of negative action. The researchers
25
concluded that most of the scales are made up of negative action items. They further noted that
the behavior-rating scales were devoid of actions questions that are based on observations,
hence the nature of majority of items listed on the rating scales were highly inferential. Thus, it
was concluded by Hosp and colleagues that the utility of behavior rating scales is very limited
and microscopic in nature.
Universal screening is extremely vital in identifying students who are experiencing
emotional and behaviors disorder right from the onset of an early age (Hintze, 2005).
Systematic screening for behavior disorders is beneficial to externalize as well as internalize the
various wide ranging spectrums of behavioral disorders using a multiple gating approach.
Monitoring behavioral progress is an essential component in the context of positive behavioral
support, behavior-intervention plans as it gives proper analysis of the work done in this regard
and how much work still needs to done. The primary method for keeping track of the progress
is by directly observing the students with cognitive and social disabilities. Many researchers
argue that direct observations is a reliable and genuine approach as there is a greater scope for
increased technical adequacy, sensitivity, utility, and social validity
Mires and Lee (2017) have well documented that poor academic characteristics like low
IQ, poor reading and comprehension skills, consistently poor grades in science and Maths
insinuate that student has academic difficulties that could possibly be linked to his mental
impairment and disabilities. Students in the school setting can be actively managed by the right
guidance and instructions from the teachers. It is understandable that teachers also need to be
pre-equipped with necessary and requisite teaching methodologies as teaching in such a
scenario can be extremely challenging as well as demanding. However, on a promising note,
interventions from teachers can yield maximum effective results as the students tend to spend
26
maximum time of their day in the classroom. Moreover, it is imperative to note that students
with ADHD often show heightened degree of symptoms in the learner setting as they find
following instructions, paying attention and forming social bonds with peers, hence school-
based interventions has potential to be most beneficial for students to overcome their
neurological impairments associated with ADHD. Furthermore researchers exclaim that school-
home communication is also extremely pivotal in providing a holistic and overall development
of students with ADHD, Daily Behavior report card can help in observing and analyzing the
dynamics between the progress at school and at home.
Positive home school collaboration is extremely significant however it is being observed
that it can be elusive (Henderson, 1987). The DBRC is just not effective to record the
observations and progress at school, but it also helps in promotion of effective homeschool
communication by facilitating teacher feedback to students on how they can improve their
classroom behavior. Researchers have demonstrated that DBRCs can be used by both special
and general educators alike and it can be used for wide range of students having varying degree
of disabilities and age groups.
DBRC has other advantages as it is quite user friendly does not involve complex
calculations and can be adapted for both paper and digital forms. However, there might be
certain caveat as the feasibility and utility can be compromised if the target student is dealing
with other important issues other than ADHD, serious health problems, in appropriate
dependence of drugs and intoxicating substances. It is highly recommended that parental
support should comprise an integral part of DBRC behavioral intervention.
27
Response Dependent Fading System
The response-dependent fading system operates through the use of prompts aimed at
reinforcing a certain wanted behavior. In the context of learners with ADHD and cognitive
developmental delays, the system can be used to teach the students how to be attentive and
minimize disruptive behaviors. In other words, the children with ADHD and developmental
delays are taught new behaviors using prompts. However, the response dependent fading system
requires the instructor to systematically fade or withdraw these prompts so that the learner can
perform taught skills or behaviors independently. Learners with ADHD are often prone to
distractions, impulsivity, and forgetfulness. Consequently, teachers can use verbal, visual,
model, gestural, and physical response prompts and reinforcements to help learners stick to a
task, acquire new behavior, and minimize destructive incidents. Therefore, the response-
dependent fading system involves decreasing the level of assistance given to the learner to a less
intrusive prompt to ensure that the student does not become overly dependent on response
prompts. The steps in this system involve identifying the behavior that needs to be taught or
minimized and the level of response or prompt required for the learner to complete tasks or
behave in a certain way and identifying the fading process and a criterion that indicates a faked
response. The response-dependent fading strategy is thus effective in decreasing levels of
teacher prompts and providing independence in behavioral modification efforts (Williams,
2012).
The effectiveness of response-dependent fading system has been evaluated in different
contexts among students with learning disabilities. For instance, 20th and 21st-century scholars
such as Fox et al. (1986) and Gilbertson et al. (2007) used the fading strategy as an intervention
for socially withdrawn preschoolers. Although the response-dependent fading system was
28
effective in decreasing the dependence on teacher prompts to form social interactions, it was
unclear whether the response prompts and reinforcements were eliminated. A different study
asserts that the response-independent fading system cannot, however, be used in isolation in
cases related to learners with disabilities (Odom et al., 1992). Nevertheless, there are minimal
studies that examine the effectiveness of the noted fading system within the confines of learners
with ADHD, and the effectiveness of the system when combined with other interventions such
as the daily behavior report cards. Therefore, there is a need for current studies on the
effectiveness of the system among ADHD learners.
Furthermore, it is observed that in order to increase success during fading, it is vital to
talk with parents, teachers and target students. At the same time, other opportunities &
experiences should also be planned to allow student access adult attention. Small celebrations
can also be held that further boosts the target student to help him get away with the fading
process (Harris & Fox, 1990).
29
CHAPTER 3
METHODS
Participants
The target student of the study was a six-year-old male, Bobby, who had a diagnosis of
Developmental Delay and was in the process of being evaluated for Attention-Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Bobby was in the first grade of a rurally-located elementary
school in northeast Tennessee. The criteria for selecting the target participant were: 1) the
student should have been enrolled in and currently attending a public or private school
classroom in grades kindergarten through 4th grade; 2) the teacher reported that the child had
difficulty attending to/engaging in 1 or more academic tasks; 3) the target student attended
school regularly; and, 4) that the student’s parents must have also provided informed consent
for participation. All of the preceding selection conditions were met.
Bobby was served primarily in a special education classroom and instructed by Ms.
Larkin and typically assisted by 1 to 2 paraprofessionals. Because Ms. Larkin was serving as the
intervention agent and the study involved collecting interview data about Bobby from her (see
Measures section below), the East Tennessee State University Institutional Review Board
determined that she be considered a participant in this study. Informed consent was therefore
obtained also from Ms Larkin.
Ms. Larkin indicated that Bobby had exhibited several behavior challenges that
interfered with his ability to receive instruction, participate in the learning activities and/or
distracted the teacher and other students. These behaviors included the following: 1) frequently
interrupting and intruding on others conversation, activities and possessions; 2) difficulty
staying focused on a task and following directions; 3) extremely distracted by non-task stimuli
30
occurring in the environment; 4) frequently talking with other students; 5) drawing on his paper
inappropriately, 6) refusal to follow instructions; 7) getting out of his seat frequently; and, 8)
engaging in non-task activities in areas of the classroom that were off limits during reading
instruction.
Setting
The study setting was a special education classroom located in a public school in rural,
northeastern Tennessee. The classroom had a total of 10 first grade students, each of whom was
approximately six years old. The class was supervised by the primary instructor Ms. Larkin and
her paraprofessionals. The classroom consisted of a half round table at the front of the room
along with six seated desks in the middle and back of the classroom. There were several other
activity areas around the classroom such as a computer area for instruction and playing games.
The study was conducted in the table/desk area during reading instruction. The instructional day
was between 8:00 AM to 3:00 PM, however, the observation and intervention procedures were
employed during the first instructional activity of the day, Reading, between 8:30 AM to 9:00
AM. To transition students from one activity to other activities, Ms. Larkin had a paper posted
on the wall that had the names and roll numbers of each student. The number referred to the
position of the line that students were to arrange themselves in moving to another activity or
lunch during the day. Before an activity Ms. Larkin typically verbally stated the students’
expectations in behavior and instruction and students were encouraged to raise their hands if
they had any questions regarding the subject matter.
Materials
The primary material for the study consisted of the Daily Behavior Report Card (e.g.,
Vannest, Burke, Sauber, Davis, & Cole, 2011). Such report cards are tailored to the specific
31
student and activity context but typically consist of a list of one or more expected behaviors that
are simply but clearly stated in behavioral terms, and some type of rating scale to indicate
whether the student engaged in the specified behaviors, the degree to which he/she did so (some
type of rating scale or pictorial code) during each relevant activity. In the current study,
Bobby’s behavior report card consisted of a card approximately 8.3 x 11.7 inches that listed
identifying information at the top of the card including the student’s name, the teacher’s name,
the date, and the classroom. The next section of the card listed three positive behaviors that Ms
Larkin has identified as relevant to Bobby’s engagement in the reading task: 1) Follow
instruction; 2) Staying in seat; and, 3) Raise hand to speak or ask for help. Below each behavior
was a 3 point rating scale that consisted of a frowning face, a neutral face, and a smiling face
below which were the numbers 1, 2, and 3 a simple graphic figure illustrating each behavior.
An actual example of Bobby’s behavior report card is included in Appendix A.
Measures
Dependent variableAcademic Engaged Time
The dependent variable in this study was the amount of Academic Engaged Time or AET
(Walker & Severson, 1992) exhibited by Bobby during the daily reading instructional activity.
The definition and measurement of AET was that contained in the direct observation component
of the Systematic Screening for Behavior Disorders/SSBD (Walker & Severson, 1992). Briefly
the SSBD defines AET as the amount of actual time a student spends engaged, attending to, and
working on relevant academic material. The student is: 1) appropriately engaged in working on
assigned academic material that is geared to his/her ability & skill levels; 2) attending to
material & task; 3) making appropriate motor responses (writing, computing); 4) asking for
32
assistance (where appropriate) in acceptable manner; 5) interacting with teacher or classmates
about academic matters; or, 6) listening to teacher instructions & directions.
Examples of AET included performing the tasks, completing his worksheet, rising hand
to ask for help or asking questions. Non-examples of AET included playing with his materials,
scribbling or drawing on his desk or paper, not following classroom/activity rules such as not
raising hands.
As specified in the SSBD direct observation manual, AET was measured using duration
recording. Observers used the stopwatch function of their iPad or smartphone to record Bobby’s
AET each day. The observer(s) entered the classroom just prior to the reading activity and
began recording AET once the teacher has signaled the beginning of the reading activity and
began instruction. When Bobby’s behavior alternately matched or deviated from the AET
definition, the observers started and stopped their stopwatches, respectively, for the length of
the reading activity or for a maximum of 20 minutes. At the end of the observation the
observer(s) noted the total duration of AET on an observation summary form as well as the
participant code number, date of the observation, the start and stop clock time of the observation
(so as to determine the percentage of instructional time that Bobby engaged in AET), and the
observer name(s) and inter-observer agreement information (when such checks occurred).
Observations were recorded during the first instructional activity of the day, reading, that lasted
between 8:30 AM to 9:30 AM.
Interobserver Agreement (IOA)
During this study the principal investigator served as the primary observer. At various
times three other different observers assisted in collecting IOA data. The primary observer and
each of the secondary observers trained on the AET observation definition and duration
33
recording procedure prior to any data collection. Training consisted of reading and reviewing
the definition and examples and non-examples of AET and practicing by using the definition
and recording procedure by observing the AET practice videos from the SSBD (Walker &
Severson, 1992). Prior to actual data collection observers had to attain a minimum of 80%
agreement with each other over three practice observations. Once baseline was begun, IOA was
periodically assessed between the primary observer and one of the secondary observers by
simultaneously but independently observing Bobby during the reading activity. Observers
stood or sat several feet apart. The primary observer signaled the beginning and end of the
observation by manually gesturing at the secondary observer. At the beginning and then the
ending of the session, the observers noted the clock time (beginning and end) and the total
number of minutes and seconds that they respectively recorded Bobby as being academically
engaged. To calculate IOA, the observers converted the minutes and seconds to total seconds of
AET, divided the smaller total of AET seconds by the larger total, and multiplied that dividend
by 100 to yield a percentage of agreement.
Functional Analysis Screening Tool
To further define the child participant’s characteristics the Functional Analysis
Screening Tool or FAST (Iwata & DeLeon, 1995) was administered by the principal investigator
to Ms Larkin to estimate the possible function(s) of the Bobby’s behavior. The FAST is an
instrument that helps determine the number of factors or variables that may trigger or maintain
the occurrence of problem behaviors demonstrated by the target students. The FAST is
composed of 2 major sections. The initial section seeks information about the role of the
informant vis a vis the child (teacher, parent, caregiver etc.). The second portion is a series of 27
statements about the specific contextual features in which the challenging behavior might occur.
34
The informant is asked to give a Yes/No response to each of these statement in terms of whether
the behavior occurs in that specific situation (e.g. the behavior often occurs when he/she has not
had attention, the behavior occurs when you tell the child he/she cannot have a specific item, &
he/she often engages in other annoying behavior that produce access to preferred items or
activities). FAST scores are grouped into 5 behavior function clusters: 1) social reinforcement-
attention; 2) social reinforcement access to specific activities/items; 3) social reinforcement
escape; 4) automatic reinforcement – sensory stimulation; and, 5) automatic reinforcement –
pain attenuation. The cluster of statements that receives the most “Yes” responses is predicted
to be the primary function of the behavior challenge. For Bobby Ms. Larkin’s responses on the
FAST indicated that “social reinforcement attention and social reinforcement access to
specific activities/items were the most likely functions of his inattentive/off task behavior.
Social Validity
After the completion of the study, the social validity of the intervention and its effects
were evaluated in two ways. The teacher, Ms. Larkin, was interviewed by the principal
investigator and his thesis advisor, the interview was in two parts. First, the Intervention Rating
Profile or IRP (Witt & Elliot, 1985) was administered to Ms Larkin by the principal
investigator., The teacher version of the IRP scale consists of 15 positive statements about the
intervention (e.g., “the DBRC is an acceptable intervention for the child’s problem behavior”,
“Most teachers would find DBRC appropriate for behavior problems”, “the DBRC was
effective in changing in the child’s problem behavior”).The teacher rates the statements on a 6-
point scale of “Strongly Agree” (6) to “Strongly Disagree” (1).
Once the teacher completed the IRP, the principal investigator and faculty advisor
informally interviewed Ms Larkin, asking her to expand on the feasibility, effectiveness and
35
appropriateness of the DBRC. Those responses were written down by the principal investigator
and faculty advisor.
Procedures
Baseline. During baseline Ms Larkin was asked to simply engage in her typical instructional
and behavior management procedures with Bobby and the other students. The reading
instructional activity typically was begun at 8:20 am and ended at 9:30 am. No other
intervention was applied at this time. Only AET observations and IOA were conducted during
this time
Intervention – Daily Behavior Report Card/DBRC 1. Following the last baseline session, the
principal investigator met with Ms Larkin to discuss and review the DBRC form and
procedures. The DBRC was then implemented over the next eight sessions. Before the
commencement of the session, Ms Larkin would review the card with Bobby, and
explaining/reviewing the expected behaviors listed on the DBRC. Once this briefing was over,
the card would be put on the table or desk in front of Bobby. As the lesson ended, Ms Larkin
rated Bobby’s performance during reading in terms of the behaviors listed on the DBRC
marking the appropriate face (frowning, neutral, smiley face) on the card. She praised Bobby
for instances of appropriate behavior and gave him corrective feedback on inappropriate
behaviors. If Bobby did not have any frowning faces Ms Larking rewarded him with a ticket.
When the ticket count reached 20, Bobby was awarded with a prize from the prize box, and
when he reached half the target; he got candy. And if he collected 5 tickets he received
additional free time for five minutes.
Withdrawal of DBRC: Following eight sessions of DBRC 1, the intervention was temporarily
but completely withdrawn and only Baseline conditions and observations were implemented.
36
This withdrawal was limited to only 5 sessions for two reasons. First, five sessions are the
minimum necessary to evaluate the stability/trend in a behavior. Second, Bobby’s AET initially
remained within intervention levels for the first two withdrawal sessions but then declined
quickly and substantially, well below his original baseline levels. It was considered ethically
more appropriate to re-instate the DBRC at this point since the minimum number of sessions for
evaluation of behavior trend during withdrawal had been accomplished, Bobby’s non-AET
behaviors was becoming more frequent and problematic for Ms Larkin, and the DBRC
intervention had appeared to have had very positive effects on Bobby during that first
intervention phase.
DBRC 2. After five sessions of DBRC withdrawal, the DBRC intervention was re-implemented
just as it had been done during DBRC 1.
Response-Dependent Fading of DBRC. During this phase, fading of the DBRC was begun.
Whereas during DBRC 1 and DBRC 2, the report card had been applied each school day, during
the fading phase it was implemented less often, that is, every other day. It was originally
planned that more extensive and successive phases of fading were to be accomplished (from
every other day, to every two days, three days, and so on). The DBRC was to be further reduced
in this stepwise fashion if Bobby maintained AET at 80% or better for three days in a row.
Unfortunately, because of various school schedule issues and changes (weather related closings
of the school district, etc.), there was not sufficient time to conduct reductions beyond the
every-other-day condition.
37
Experimental Design
The design of the study was a single subject reversal design or A-B-A-B (Kennedy,
2005). This design involves establishing an initial baseline (nonintervention) level of the target
behavior over multiple observation sessions. Baseline is followed by successive phases of
intervention followed by withdrawal of the intervention and then reapplication of the
intervention over repeated observations during each of these phases. In this study this consisted
of the following specific phases: 1) Baseline 1, 2) DBRC 1, 3) Withdrawal of DBRC, 4) DBRC
2, and finally, 5) Fading of DBRC (Brown, 2007).
38
CHAPTER 4
RESULTS
Interobserver Agreement
The Inter-observer Agreement (IOA) was calculated for 11 of the 34 sessions or 32 % of
the entire observation series. Because the observation method involved duration recording, IOA
was calculated by dividing the smaller number of minutes/ seconds of engagement recorded by
one observer by the larger number of minutes/ seconds of engagement recorded by the second
observer and then multiplying it by 100 to derive the total percentage of agreement. The overall
mean and median IOA were 93% and 92% respectively, and the minimum and maximum were
87% and 100% respectively, a range of 13%.
The individual IOA results are enumerated as follows in Table 1:
Table 1.
Interobserver Agreement per Session
Observation session
Primary Observer
2
18:01 mins
5
14:37 mins
13
18:30 mins
18
18:11 mins
20
12:08 mins
24
18:25 mins
26
17:49 mins
29
15:23 mins
31
20:00 mins
32
17:39 mins
34
16:20 mins
Median
Range
39
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
90%
100%
1* 3* 5* 7* 9 11 13 15 17* 19* 21* 23 25 27 29* 31* 33 35
Percentage of Time
Days
* IOA check
Academic Engaged Time
Bobby’s Academic Engagement Time was calculated by dividing the total observed
engagement time by the total observation time. Figure 1 shows the daily percentage of AET for
each phase of the study. (The overall trend in AET was calculated by the quarter intersect
method (Tawney and Gast, 1984) for each phase and is shown by the dashed arrows in each
phase in Figure 1.
Baseline 1 DBRC1 Baseline2 DBRC2 Fading
Figure 1. Daily Percentage of and Trend in Bobby’s Academic Engaged Time per Phase
Looking at Fig. 1 it can be seen that during baseline Bobby’s AET displayed an overall
decreasing trend with a median of 68% and a range of 60 to 80% AET. After the first
40
application of the DBRC, Bobby’s AET exhibited an increasing trend with a median AET of
80% and the range was 72 to 97%. Withdrawal of the intervention during Baseline 2 was
followed initially by a brief increase in AET during the first two days. However, there was an
overall decreasing trend in AET during this phase with a median of 60% and a range of 50 to
80%. Re-application of the intervention during DBRC 2 showed an immediate increase in
AET with an increasing trend, a median of 87 % and a range of 73 to 89%. When Fading of
DBRC was begun there was a slight decreasing trend across that phase but both the median
(83%) and range of AET (80 to 88%) remained above the targeted level of 80%.
Additional analyses were conducted to further evaluate Bobby’s AET under the
Baseline, DBRC, and Fading conditions. These analyses included: 1) the Change in Mean Level
calculated as the difference between the mean of one phase and the mean of the next adjacent
phase; and, 2) the Percentage of Overlap in data points of one phase with those of the preceding
phase. The greater the Mean Level Change and the less Overlap between phases indicate the
strength and reliability of the behavior change. Table 2 shows the data for these indices.
Table 2.
Academic Engaged Time: Change in Mean Level, and Percentage of Overlap
Change in Mean
Level
Percentage
Overlap
Baseline 1 to
DBRC 1
+12%
63%
DBRC 1 to
Baseline 2
-12%
40%
Baseline 2 to
DBRC 2
+16%
57%
DBRC 2 to Fading
-2%
100%
41
Generally the data in Table 2 show the expected direction and magnitude in Mean Level
Change per phase. AET Mean Level increased from Baseline 1 to DBRC 1 by 12% overall,
decreased from DBRC to Baseline 2 by 12%, increased again by 16% from Baseline 2 to DBRC
2 and decreased only slightly, 2%, from DBRC2 to Fading. Overlap between phases was
considerable varying from 64% between Baseline 1 and DBRC 1, 40% during DBRC 1 to
Baseline 2, 57% during Baseline 2 to DBRC 2. Overlap between DBRC and Fading was 100%;
however, this indicates that AET remained within targeted levels despite the lessened
application of the DBRC intervention, i.e., maintenance of increased AET.
DBRC Target Behaviors
Recall that Bobby could earn points for each one of the target behaviors, following
instructions, staying in his seat and raising his hand to speak or ask questions. Shown in Figure
2 are the percentage of points Bobby received for each target behavior during DBRC 1, DBRC
2 and Fading phases.
Figure 2. The Percentage of DBRC Behaviors for Which Bobby Received Points
83%
87%
96%
75%
80%
85%
90%
95%
100%
Follow instruction Staying in seat Raise hand to speak
or ask for help
Bobby's Percentage of Points for Target Behaviors
42
These data show that Bobby engaged in the appropriate behaviors somewhat differentially but
overall he engaged in each of the target behaviors at a relatively high level. Out of 54 points
possible, Bobby scored 83%, 87%, 96% respectively in all three target behaviors. The total card
collected by Bobby stood at 14 by the time this study was completed.
Social Validity
Teacher Intervention Rating Profile. The Intervention Rating Profile completed by Ms.
Larkin as a measure of the perceived effectiveness, efficiency and acceptability of the DBRC.
Overall, Ms Larkin rated the DBRC intervention very positively. Of the 15 positive statements
on the profile, the overall mean score was 5.67, with a range 4 to 6, on a scale of 1 to 6 (1
Strongly Disagree to 6 Strongly Agree). All statements received positive ratings from Ms
Larkin. Her lowest ratings were on 3 items: statement 1 (“DBRC is an acceptable intervention
for the child’s problem behavior”) which was rated “5” (Agree), statement 3 (“DBRC was
effective in changing in the child’s problem behavior”) and statement 15 (“Overall, DBRC was
beneficial for the child”) which were both rated “4” (Slightly Agree).
Participant and Normative Data for AET. To provide an additional index of social validity
we compared Bobby’s AET to that that might be expected of other students in a similar graded
range. The Systematic Screening for Behavior Disorders (Walker & Severson, 1992) provides
grade level normative data for Academic Engaged Time that is further broken down into
percentages of AET for “Normal” and “Externalizes” and “Internalizes”, the latter two groups
being those identified as potentially at risk for emotional-behavioral disorders. Table 3 presents
the Mean AET for those three normative groups in Grades 1 through 3 (Walker & Severson,
1992 Systematic Screening for Behavior Disorders: Observer Training Manual, p. 131). The
table also shows the mean percentage of AET for Bobby during each phase of the study. These
data indicate that during non-intervention phases of the study (Baseline 1 and Baseline 2)
43
Bobby’s mean percentage of AET (68%) was below that of grade level norms and similar to
that of internalizers and externalizers in the SSBD normative groups (69.71 and 62.72%,
respectively). During the intervention phases DBRC 1, DBRC 2 and Fading, Bobby’s mean
AET (81%, 85%, 83% respectively) exceeded that of the Normal, Externalizing and
Internalizing groups (75.19, 62.72, and 69.71%respectively).
Table 3.
AET Normative Data and Bobby’s AET (Percentage of AET)
Mean
Standard Deviation
Normal
75.19
14.98
Externalizers
62.72
20.37
Internalizers
69.71
17.30
Total
70.35
17.96
Bobby - Baseline
68%
0.077
Bobby – DBRC 1
81%
0.057
Bobby - Reversal
68%
0.156
Bobby – DBRC 2
85%
0.061
Bobby – Fading DBRC
83%
61
44
CHAPTER 5
DISCUSSION
The research study was aimed at better understanding the initial and maintenance effect
of the use of Daily Behavior Report Card (DBRC) intervention and fading procedure on
Academic Engaged Time (AET) of a 6 year-old boy with Developmental Delay and Attention
Deficits were evaluated with a reversal design. The target student, Bobby, was an elementary
school student identified as developmentally delayed who exhibited behavior challenges
consistent with ADHD such as frequently interrupting others, difficulty staying on a task,
refusing to follow instructions, getting out of his seat and engaging in non-task activities during
reading instruction. After establishing a baseline level of academic engagement, a Daily
Behavior Report Card was implemented with Bobby by his teacher and a reversal single subject
design was used to evaluate its effects. Compared to baseline, the DBRC substantially
increased academic engaged time (AET) to within normative levels for his age (Walker &
Severson, 1992). Furthermore, complete and abrupt removal of the DBRC quickly decreased
Bobby’s AET whereas a phase in which the DBRC was gradually removed kept AET within the
normative range. Social validity ratings by the teacher on the Intervention Rating Profile (Witt
& Elliot, 1985) indicated that overall she found the DBRC intervention to be relatively
effective, easy to use and appropriate for Bobby and his behavior.
In the previous research studies, it has been found that implementation of DBRC is
associated with increased task engagement in the case of many target students since the first
reported use of DBRC more than 40 years ago (Bailey, Wolf & Phillips, 1970). Meta analyses
have consistently reported the positive, initial intervention effects of the DBRC (e.g., Owens et
al., 2012; Pyle & Fabio, 2017). At the same time specific effects of DBRC itself are still
45
unclear as it has often been used as part of other intervention package that included additional
components such as Check in Check out (e.g., Hawken, Bundock, Kladis, O'Keeffe, & Barret,
2014) and the results have sometimes been mixed. For example, Owens et al. (2012) in a meta-
analysis reported that although 72% of the students to whom the DBRC had been applied were
found to have significant behavioral improvement, 8% had actually experienced a decline in
behavior. Hence, the specific effects noted in the current and other studies between the DBRC
engaged academic time and enhanced learning needs further research. We are currently in the
process of replicating the intervention with additional elementary aged students with attention
and task engagement problem behaviors.
In our research study, the efforts were made to begin systematically fading out the
DBRC intervention process with the purpose of reducing the student’s reliance on the DBRC,
i.e., producing more independent academic engagement, as well as reducing the teacher’s time
and effort to implement the intervention. We sought to minimize the implementation of DBRC
by cutting down the frequency of Bobby’s. Our plan was to reduce the daily use of DBRC if
Bobby reached 80% AET for three days in a row, beginning with DBRC use every other day
and gradually extending the days between use (i.e., every 2 days, every 3 days, once a week,
etc.). It was observed that when the fading process was implemented the DBRC intervention
was implemented every other day, Bobby’s AET evidenced a slight decreasing trend but
remained well within the range of his original intervention levels and within the normative AET
level as reported in the SSBD norms (Walker & Severson, 1992), i.e.,80% and above. This was
in contrast to the prior reversal phase when the DBRC was completely withdrawn and his AET
quickly decreased well below the original intervention levels. Unfortunately, we ran out of time
in the school year to continue the stepwise fading of DBRC and therefore we do not know what
46
the maintenance effects of further reductions in the DBRC would have been. Fading tactics
have been successfully used in other experimental contexts to improve the maintenance of
behavior change (e.g., Coleman, 1973; Fox, Shores, Lindeman, & Strain, 1986; Kagohora,
2011; Meyer, Hagopian, & Paclwaskyj, 1999) and in one instance to when teacher monitoring
via Check In Check Out was faded to student self-monitoring (Miller, Dufrene, Olmi,
Tingstrom & Filce, 2015). However, the brief fading phase in the present study in conjunction
with prior studies does suggest that systematic response dependent fading might very well prove
successful and future DBRC research should examine more fully this tactic
A meta-analytic study conducted by Vannest and colleagues (Vannest et al 2010),
studied the impact of DBRC on 17 target students and found that the improvement rate
difference (IRD) for all the students averaged at 0.61 with a range of -0.15 to 0.97. Though, the
research suggested that DBRC was likely to improve the performance and increases the
possibility of positive influence target behaviors of the students with trouble symptoms in
general, however the large magnitude of the variation also suggested that the DBRC
intervention cannot be deemed as an unqualified success and needs further investigation in
terms of how it can be adjusted to be more broadly effective.
Crnic, Hoffman, Gaze, & Edelbrock (2004) suggested that young students with
developmental delay have a tendency to show heightened challenges socially and behaviorally.
In contrast with the typically developing students of their age, they show greater aggression,
disobedience and defiant behavior (Ageranioti-Bélanger et al., 2012). Byrne (2015) concluded
that the use of classroom intervention technique in extended school year settings can show
positive influence on students. The students with repeated classroom interventions like DBRC
demonstrated greater improvement and progress in social aspects of their lives like talking to
47
classroom teachers and peers, raising hands for help etc. Those findings, those of previous
DBRC studies (e.g., Owens et al., 2012; Pyle & Fabio, 2017) and our findings in the current
study our study resonate with the consistency of the previous research on DBRC
implementation and its impact on AET as well as other academic aspects of students like
enhanced learning potency (Hill & Flores, 2014).
The results of the current study support the notion that use of DBRC can be a sound
intervention tool for teachers as well as students for improving and maintaining behaviors. The
teacher advocated the use of the DBRC and expressed her fidelity to the proposed intervention
program in future test studies as well. She rated the DBRC is a feasible and reliable method of
improving student engagement time and in the past studies, similar sentiments were expressed
by other teachers as well (who were primarily responsible for employment of DBRC at school)
(Jonte’C et al., 2017). The teachers were generally competent enough to collect student-
behavior data throughout the day and then evaluating it by the end of the day.
Furthermore, communication between teachers and parents/families is shown to have
positive impact on the behavioral improvement of students by the previous studies. It increases
the effectiveness of DBRC. On the similar line, it is imperative to note that BASC II is an
effective tool for assessment and identification of school-age children with emotional
disturbances and developmental issues. The basic rationale behind BASC is to gather pool of
data about the child through wide variety of sources that could range from teachers, friends,
families, historical reports and observations. Hence, prior employment of BASC is said to be
beneficial in devising the right DBRC strategy.
Chafouleas (2007) investigated the social validity of the procedure by including the
doctoral students in school psychology as independent observers and assess the feasibility of the
48
study. They were trained daily for 1.5 hours and were made to comprehensibly study the
various aspects of DBRC intervention and were periodically consulted for reviews, suggestions
and further recommendations. Furthermore, to help the teachers get better overview of the
actual implementation of the DBRC implementation, analogue scenarios describing student
classroom behavior were read and discussed. The teacher’s consent was given due consideration
while determining the timing and place of the intervention.
In addition to the social validity, several studies also studied the content validity. In
order to assess the content validity of the DBRC, academic DBRC targets and IEP goals related
to academic progress and functioning were studied (Fabiano et al., 2009; Dyke et al., 2014).
In many studies, though there has not been explicit mention of social validity, however
teacher feedback was consistently taken in majority of the test studies on their DBRC
performance. These feedbacks acted detrimental in establishing social validity as teachers were
always prior consented before the implementation of DBRC (Chafouleas et al., 2007; Fabiano et
al., 2009) Furthermore, in many of the case studies, home-school communication has also been
given due consideration, so that parents can also engage in the DBRC implementation (Mires
et al., 2008; Owens et al., 2012) Such measures increase the social validity of the tests, even if
not explicitly stated.
Furthermore, in many cases, the students were also interviewed at the end of the study to
get their feedbacks and to give them opportunity to expresses the things that they liked or
disliked in the study. The students were generally asked if they felt more confident about
themselves, upon the implementation of DBRC and if they wanted to continue using the DBRC
implementation.
49
Fading is considered an essential phase when a child gradually learns to maintain high
AET even without application or implementation of DBRC. In a comprehensive fading study,
conducted by Rock and Thead (2007) the fading condition was divided into five distinct phases
that were carried out for a period of two weeks. The goal of the fading process was to gradually
reduce the students’ use of DBRC intervention. The first four phases 1, 2, 3, and 4 lasted for
three days each and the last phase lasted for only two days. Over the course of 14 days, the
students were students were instructed to continuously and silently assess their performance
until the end of the period to determine whether or not they had met their academic and
behavioral goals.
The meta-analysis conducted by Pyle and Fabio (2017) presented the results that DBRC
is an effective intervention tool for students with ADHD in single-study cases. The meta- study
comprised of study data and results complied from over 40 single-case test studies.
The research study purported the idea that DBRC is known to play detrimental role in
changing student’s target behaviors by a significant margin of 30 base points from baseline to
intervention. The study acknowledges that quantification of effects across single-case studies in
a meta-analysis is an evolving area within the field of intervention research. In order to gauge
the effectiveness of DBRC as an intervention tool across the varying incidences of single-case
studies, graphed time-series data is employed visually as well as quantitatively. The study
argues that as of now there is no pertinent gold standard for the calculation of magnitude of
effect in single-case research, however graphical analysis is still the most sought after
methodology.
The meta-study ran parallel to our study as the participants who were carefully selected
for inclusion in the study was already identified with ADHD. All the participants were lesser
50
than 18 years old; the studies used standardized ADHD rating scales, and employed DBRC as
their primary intervention tool. Furthermore, the studies included in meta-analysis examined
observation of disruptive or on task behavior as primary outcome, even in our case study; we
examined the target behaviors displayed by Bobby as the primary observatory methodology of
the assessment of his condition. The common outcome variables that were identified are
percentage of time spent by target students actively engaged in their task, number of changes in
activity, percentage of time children spent demonstrating hyperactive symptoms. In order to
better understand the recorded observations, the results were converted to percentages. In
addition to it, all activities were classified as on-task or off-task. On task activities in the
metastudy as well as the case study included raising hands properly when one wants to ask
questions, staying on desk and completing class assignments on time whereas the off-task
activities included staying off the desks, staying distracted, and disturbing others. For the
reliability, Interobserver agreement was used.
In a nutshell, the study supports the use of DBRC as the intervention tool, and our study
also suggests the same that use of DBRC should be continued and should certainly be
recommended for treating students with mild to aggressive ADHD. However, in case of the
meta-study the effectiveness of DRC has been found to be very high, unlike our case study,
where we managed to get only moderate success. However, in the light of the findings of the
meta-study it is undeniable that DBRC has emerged to be one of the most sought after
intervention tool and its implementation should be continued with adequate supervision to
monitor the impact of different variables such as school settings, home-school communication
etc.
51
Limitations and the Future Research
As it is previously stated, study of just one target population, is the first and foremost
limitation of the research study. However, it is unclear how a different subject might have
reacted to similar settings. Thus, it is imperative to increase the scale of this search study with
greater target population to generalize the findings. The second limitation was the location and
setting of the intervention. The use of DBRC was carried out in classes, where there used to be
many compound variables like noise, visual distraction etc. The implications of these variables
were not taken into consideration. It is possible that these compound variables might have
affected the target behavior of the student in any form that went unnoticed. For instance, In the
second Baseline period, there was a sharp downfall of AET on the third day from 80% to 63%.
This could be attributed to any such factor.
The present literature review in the context of developmental delay and disorder is still
very limited, and the use of DBRC as an effective intervention tool need to further assessed. In
future, similar research studies should be conducted at a much larger scale to have an unbiased
generalization of the viewpoint. Future research might also consider gender of the students as a
primary research topic. It would be interesting to see if gender of students has a correlation with
effectiveness of DBRC. Furthermore, most of the literature review about ADHD and other
developmental delay is about younger populated who are aged 18 or below, further research
should be done to study the effectiveness of DBRC for adults as well. The scope can be
widened by conducting a longitudinal study where the same sample population would be studies
over the years and how after receiving intervention methodologies at a younger age, they
perform at later stages of their life. On the other hand, because the volume of literature is still
52
gaining momentum, a latitudinal study should be undertaken to see if the results of
implementation of DBRC varies with age.
53
REFERENCES
Ageranioti-Bélanger, S., Brunet, S., D’Anjou, G., Tellier, G., Boivin, J., & Gauthier, M. (2012).
Behaviour disorders in children with an intellectual disability. Paediatrics & Child
Health, 17, 8488. doi: 10.1093/pch/17.2.84
Additude Editors (2014). The daily report card: Your secret weapon for better classroom
behavior. Attitude. Retrieved from additudemag.com/daily-report-card-to-improve-
adhd-classroom-behavior/.
Benazzi, L., Horner, R., & Good, R. (2006). Effects of behavior support team composition on
the technical adequacy and contextual fit of behavior support plans. Journal of Special
Education, 40, 160–170.
Biederman, J., Faraone, S., Milberger, S., Guite, J., Mick, E., Chen, L., ...& Spencer, T. (1996).
A prospective 4-year follow-up study of attention-deficit hyperactivity and related
disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(5), 437-446.
Brown, R. T. (2007). Galen: Developer of the Reversal Design? The Behavior Analyst, 30(1),
31–35.
Byrne, D. C. (2015). Investigating the effectiveness of research-based classroom management
in an extended school-year program. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of
Connecticut, Mansfield, CT.
Burke, M., & Vannest, K. (2008). Behavioral-progress monitoring using the electronic daily
behavioral report card (e-DBRC) system. Preventing School Failure. 52(3), 51-59.
Crnic, K., Hoffman, C., Gaze, C., & Edelbrock, C. (2004). Understanding the emergence of
behavior problems in young children with developmental delays. Infants & Young
Children, 17, 223– 235. doi: 10.1097/00001163–200407000–00004
54
Chafouleas, S.M., Riley-Tillman, T.C., & McDougal, J.I. (2002). Good, Bad, or in-between:
How does the daily behavior report card rate? Psychology in the Schools, 39, 157-169.
Chafouleas, S. M., Riley-Tillman, T. C., &Sassu, K. A. (2006). Acceptability and reported use
of daily behavior report cards among teachers. Journal of Positive Behavior
Interventions, 8, 174–182.
Coleman, R. G. (1973). A procedure for fading from experimenter-school-based to parent-
home-based control of classroom behavior. Journal of School Psychology, 11(1), 71-79.
DuPaul, G., & Stoner, G. (2003). ADHD in the schools assessment and intervention strategies.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Dyck, G.,& Piek, J. (2014). Developmental Delays in Children with ADHD. Journal of
Attention Disorders, 12(1), 221-234.
Fox. J, Shores, R., Lindeman, D., & Strain, P. (1986). Maintaining social initiations of
withdrawn handicapped and nonhandicapped preschoolers through response-dependent
fading tactic. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 14(3), 387-396.
Frafjord-Jacobson, K. L., Hanson, A. C., McLaughlin, T. F., Stansell, A., & Howard, V. F.
(2013). Daily report cards: A recommended intervention in the schools. International
Journal of Basic and Applied Science, 1(3), 461-472.
Gilbertson, D., Witt, J. C., Singletary, L. L., & VanDerHeyden, A. (2007). Supporting teacher
use of interventions: Effects of response-dependent performance feedback on teacher
implementation of a math intervention. Journal of Behavioral Education, 16(4), 311-
326.
55
Greene, R. W., Beszterczey, S. K., Katzenstein, T., Park, K., & Goring, J. (2002). Are students
with ADHD more stressful to teach? Patterns of teacher stress in an elementary school
sample. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 10(2), 79-89.
Harris-Solomon, A. & Fox, J.J. (November 1990). Compliance training and response-dependent
fading of intervention procedures: A case study of the treatment of severe, chronic non-
compliance. Paper presented at the 14th Annual TECBD National Conference on Severe
Behavior Disorders of Children and Youth, Tempe, AZ.
Hawken, L. S., Bundock, K., Kladis, K., O'Keeffe, B., & Barret, C. A. (2014). Systematic
review of the Check-In, Check-Out intervention for students at risk for emotional and
behavioral disorders. Education and Treatment of Children, 37 (4), 635-658
Hill, D. A., & Flores, M. M. (2014). Modeling positive behavior interventions and supports for
preservice teachers. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 16, 93–101. doi:
10.1177/1098300713478665
Hintze, J.M. (2005). Psychometrics of direct observation. School Psychology Review, 34, 507-
519.
Hosp, J. L., Howell, K. W., & Hosp, M. K. (2003). Characteristics of behavior rating scales:
Implication for practice in assessment and behavior support. Journal of Positive
Behavior Interventions, 4, 201-208.
Hunderson, P. (1987). The evidence continues to grow: parent involvement achievement.
Columbia, MD National Committee for Citizens in Education.
Iwata, B.A., & DeLeon, I.G. (1995). The Functional Analysis Screening Tool (FAST), the
Florida Center on Self-Injury, University of Florida, Gainesville, Fl.
56
Iznardo, M., Rogers, M., Volpe, R.J., Labelle, P.R., & Robaey, P . (2017). The effectiveness of
daily behavior report cards for children with ADHD: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Attention Disorders. 1-14 DOI: 10.1177/1087054717734646
Jonte', C, T., & Hill, D. (2017). Using Daily Behavior Report Cards during Extended School
Year Services for Young Students with Intellectual and Developmental
Disabilities. Education and Treatment of Children, 40(4), 525-546.
Kagohara, D.M. (2011). Three students with developmental disabilities learn to operate an iPod
to access age-appropriate entertainment videos. Journal of Behavior Education, 20, 33-
43.
Kennedy, C. H. (2005). Single-case designs for educational research. Boston: Allyn &
Bacon
Koul, R., Al-Yahmedy, M., & Al-Futaisi, A. (2012). Evaluation Children with Global
Developmental Delay: A Prospective Study at Sultan Qaboos University Hospital,
Oman. Oman Medical Journal, 27(4), 310–313. http://doi.org/10.5001/omj.2012.76
Loe, I. M., & Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children with
ADHD. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(6), 643-654.
Martens, B. K., Witt, J. C., Elliott, S. N., & Darveaux, D. X. (1985). Teacher judgments
concerning the acceptability of school-based interventions. Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 16(2), 191.
Meyer, E.A., Hagopian, L.P., & Paclawskyj, T.R. (1999). A function-based treatment for school
refusal behavior using shaping and fading. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
20(6), 401-410
57
Miller, L.M., Dufrene, B.A., Olmi D.J., Tingstrom, D., & Filce, H. (2015). Self-monitoring as a
viable fading option in check-in/check-out. Journal of School Psychology, 53, 121-135.
Mires, C., & Lee, D. (2017). Calvin won’t sit down! the daily behavior report card: a practical
technique to change student behavior and increase school-home communication. Beyond
Behavior, 26(2), 89-95.
Odom, S. L., Chandler, L. K., Ostrosky, M., McConnell, S. R., & Reaney, S. (1992). Fading
teacher prompts from peer-initiation interventions for young children with disabilities.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(2), 307-317.
Owens, J., Holdaway, A., Zoromski, A., Evans, S., Himawan, L., Girio-Herrera, E., & Murphy,
C. (2012). Incremental Benefits of a Daily Report Card Intervention Over Time for
Youth With Disruptive Behavior. Behavior Therapy, 43(4), 848-861.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2012.02.002
Plavnick, J.B., & Ferreri, S.J. (2013). Single-Case Experimental Designs in Educational
Research: A Methodology for Causal Analyses in Teaching and Learning. Educational
Psychology Review. 25(4). 549-569.
Pyle, K., & Fabiano, G. A. (2017). Daily report card intervention and attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of single-case studies. Exceptional
Children, 83(4), 378-395.
Rapport, M. D., Scanlan, S. W., & Denney, C. B. (1999). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder and scholastic achievement: A model of dual developmental pathways. The
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(8), 1169-1183.
58
Rock, M. L., &Thead, B. K. (2007). The effects of fading a strategic self-monitoring
intervention on students’ academic engagement, accuracy, and productivity. Journal of
Behavioral Education, 16(4), 389-412.
Tawney, J.W., & Gast, D. L. (1984). Single Subject Research in Special Education. Columbus,
OH: Merrill
Timm, M.A. (1978). The effects of systematic, response-dependent fading and leaning
procedures on the maintenance of child-child interactions. Dissertation Abstracts.
George Peabody College for Teachers.
Turco, T.L.; Elliott, S.N. (1986). Assessment of students' acceptability ratings of teacher-
initiated interventions for classroom misbehavior. Journal of School Psychology, 24(3),
277-283.
Vannest, K.J., Burke, M.D., Sauber, S.B., Davis, J.L., & Cole, R.D. (2011). Daily behavior
report cards as evidence-based practice for teachers. Beyond Behavior, 20(2), 13-21
Vannest, K.J, Davis, J.L., Davis, C.R., Mason, B.A., & Burke, M.D. (2010). Effective
intervention for behavior with a daily behavior report card: A meta-analysis. School
Psychology Review, 39(4), 654-672
Volpe, R. J., DuPaul, G. J., DiPerna, J. C., &Jitendra, A. K. (2006). Attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder and scholastic achievement: A model of mediation via academic
enablers. School Psychology Review, 35(1), 47.
Volpe, R. J., &Fabiano, G. A. (2013). Daily Behavior Report Cards: An Evidence-Based
System of Assessment and Intervention. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
59
Walker, H. M., & Severson, H. H. (1992). Systematic screening for behavior disorders (SSBD).
Sopris West, 1140 Boston Ave., Longmont, CO 80501.
Williams, K. L., Noell, G. H., Jones, B. A., & Gansle, K. A. (2012). Modifying students’
behaviors using an electronic daily behavior report card. Child & Family Behavior
Therapy, 34, 269-289. Doi: 10.1080/07317107.2012.732844
Wright, J. (2017). Classroom Behavior Report Card Resource Book. New York: School-Based
Intervention Team (SBIT). http://www.interventioncentral.org,
jim@jimwrightonline.com
60
Appendices
Appendix A
Student Daily Behavior Report Card
Student:__________________
Date:
Teacher:
Classroom:
Directions: Please rate the student each day on the behavioral items below:
Follow instruction
1
2
3
Staying in seat
1
2
3
Raise hand to speak or ask for help
Teacher Feedback:
Student have been reward: Yes No
1
2
3
61
Appendix B
Systematic Screening for Behavior Disorders (SSBD)
Academic Engaged Time
AET refers to the amount of actual time a student spends actively engaged, attending to, and
working on relevant academic material.
DEFINITION of Academic Engaged Time (AE)
The student is:
1. appropriately engaged in working on assigned academic material that is geared to his/her
ability & skill levels.
2. attending to material & task
3. making appropriate motor responses (writing, computing),
4. asking for assistance (where appropriate) in acceptable manner,
5. interacting with teacher or classmates about academic matters, or
6. listening to teacher instructions & directions
NON EXAMPLES of Academic Engaged Time (NOT)
Non-examples of AET include:
1. not attending to task
2. breaking classroom rules (out of seat, talking out, disturbing others, etc.), OR
3. daydreaming
When AET is to be observed:
AET is observed and recorded during 15 – 20 minute independent seatwork periods wherein the
student is expected to be working on assigned academic material(s).
RECORDING INSTRUCTIONS (paper form version)
1. Select a seatwork period in which at least 15 20 minutes of class time has been
allocated for independent seatwork on an assigned academic task.
2. Note the hour and minute that you begin observing and record it on the AET form.
3. Record the amount of time the pupil displays behavior consistent with the definition.
Let the stopwatch run when the pupil is academically engaged and turn it off when
he/she is not. Restart it when the pupil is again academically engaged. Repeat this
procedure throughout the recording interval.
4. Record the time you stop on the AET form.
5. Compute percent AET b dividing the time on the stopwatch by the total time observed
(e.g., 15 minutes) and multiplying by 100. Convert time observed and time on the
stopwatch to seconds (15 minutes = 900 seconds). Note: The two classroom
observations of a single student should not be scheduled in the same week. However, if
62
it is necessary to do so, schedule the observations as far apart as possible (e.g., Monday
and Friday).
6. Record the data from the two classroom observations on the AET recording form.
7. Average the two AET observation sessions to obtain an overall AET score. You can do
this by averaging the two AET times or by adding the stopwatch times together for the
two sessions and dividing by the total time of the two observation sessions.
Walker, Hill M.; Severson, Herbert H.; (1992). Systematic Screening for Behavior Disorders
(SSBD). Second Edition, Oregon Research Inst., Eugene.; University of Oregon Eugene. Sopris
West.
63
Academic Engaged Time (AET) Summary Form
Student: Teacher: Observer 1:
*(Use Codes for Student & Teacher) Observer 2:
Activity: Time Begin: Time End:
Was this an Inter-observer Agreement Session? _____Yes _____No
Primary Observer
# Minutes:Seconds Recorded that student was AET
# Minutes Observed (Time Ended – Time Began)
% Time Student AET: (# Minutes AET/#Minutes Observed) x 100
2
nd
Observer
# Minutes:Seconds Recorded that student was AET
# Minutes Observed (Time Ended – Time Began)
% Time Student AET: (# Minutes AET/#Minutes Observed) x 100
Example of AET summary & % AET calculation:
AET Summary
Observation began at 10:00 & Ended at 10:20 = 20 minutes (1200 seconds)
Observer 1 records 10 minutes: 30 seconds of AET (or 630 seconds)
Observation time was 20 minutes (or 1200 seconds)
Observer 1 % Time AET = 630/1200 = 0.525 x 100 or 52.5 % AET
Observer 2 records 12 minutes: 15 seconds of AET (or 735 seconds)
Observation time was 20 minutes (or 1200 seconds)
Observer 2 % Time AET = 735/1200 = 0.6125 x 100 or 61.3% AET
Interobserver agreement (IOA)
Divide smaller recorded time in AET by larger recorded time in AET
Smaller time in AET = 630 seconds
Larger Time in AET = 735
630/735 = 0.857 x 100 = 85.7% agreement (IOA)
64
Appendix C
FUNCTIONAL ASSESSMENT SCREENING TOOL (FAST)
Name: ___________________________________________ Age: __________________ Date: _________
Behavior Problem: _____________________________________________________________________________
Informant: ________________________________________ Interviewer: ______________________________
To the Interviewer: The Functional Analysis Screening Tool (FAST) is designed to identify a number of factors
that may influence the occurrence of problem behaviors. It should be used only as an initial screening toll and
as part of a comprehensive functional assessment or analysis of problem behavior. The FAST should be
administered to several individuals who interact with the person frequently. Results should then be used as the
basis for conducting direct observations in several different contexts to verify likely behavioral functions, clarify
ambiguous functions, and identify other relevant factors that may not have been included in this instrument.
To the Informant: After completing the section on “Informant-Person Relationship,” read each of the numbered
items carefully. If a statement accurately describes the person’s behavior problem, circle “Yes.” If not, circle “No.”
If the behavior problem consists of either self-injurious behavior or “repetitive stereotyped behaviors,” begin with
Part I. However, if the problem consists of aggression or some other form of socially disruptive behavior , such
as property destruction or tantrums, complete only Part II.
Informant-Person Relationship
Indicate your relationship to the person: _____Parent _____Teacher/Instructor _____Residential
Staff _____Other
How long have you known the person? _____Years _____Months
Do you interact with the person on a daily basis? _____Yes _____No
If “Yes,” how many hours per day?__________ If “No,” how many hours per week? _________
In what situations do you typically observe the person? (Mark all that apply)
_____Self-care routines _____Academic skills training _____Meals _____When (s)he has nothing to do
_____Leisure activities _____Work/vocational training _____Evenings _____Other:___________________
Part I. Social Influences on Behavior
1.
The behavior usually occurs in your presence or in the presence of others Yes No
2.
The behavior usually occurs soon after you or others interact with him/her in some way, such as delivering
an instruction or reprimand, walking away from (ignoring) the him/her, taking away a “preferred” item,
requiring him/her to change activities, talking to someone else in his/her presence, etc.
Yes No
3.
The behavior often is accompanied by other “emotional” responses, such as yelling or crying Yes No
Complete Part II if you answered “Yes” to item 1, 2, or 3. Skip Part II if you answered “No” to all three items in Part I.
Part II. Social Reinforcement
4.
The behavior often occurs when he/she has not received much attention Yes No
5.
When the behavior occurs, you or others usually respond by interacting with the him/her in some way (e.g.,
comforting statements, verbal correction or reprimand, response blocking, redirection)
Yes No
6.
(S)he often engages in other annoying behaviors that produce attention Yes No
7.
(S)he frequently approaches you or others and/or initiates social interaction Yes No
8.
The behavior rarely occurs when you give him/her lots of attention Yes No
9.
The behavior often occurs when you take a particular item away from him/her or when you terminate a
preferred leisure activity (If “Yes,” identify:________________________________________________)
Yes No
65
10.
The behavior often occurs when you inform the person that (s)he cannot have a certain item or cannot
engage in a particular activity. (If “Yes,” identify:___________________________________________)
Yes No
11.
When the behavior occurs, you often respond by giving him/her a specific item, such as a favorite toy,
food, or some other item. (If “Yes,” identify:_______________________________________________)
Yes No
12.
(S)he often engages in other annoying behaviors that produce access to preferred items or activities. Yes No
13.
The behavior rarely occurs during training activities or when you place other types of demands on him/her.
(If “Yes,” identify the activities: ____self-care ____academic ____work ____other)
Yes No
14.
The behavior often occurs during training activities or when asked to complete tasks. Yes No
15.
(S)he often is noncompliant during training activities or when asked to complete tasks. Yes No
16.
The behavior often occurs when the immediate environment is very noisy or crowed. Yes No
17.
When the behavior occurs, you often respond by giving him/her brief “break from an ongoing task. Yes No
18.
The behavior rarely occurs when you place few demands on him/her or when you leave him/her alone. Yes No
Part III. Nonsocial (Automatic)Reinforcement
19.
The behavior occurs frequently when (s)he is alone or unoccupied Yes No
20.
The behavior occurs at relatively high rates regardless of what is going on in his/her immediate
surrounding environment
Yes No
21.
(S)he seems to have few known reinforcers or rarely engages in appropriate object manipulation or “play”
behavior.
Yes No
22.
(S)he is generally unresponsive to social stimulation. Yes No
23.
(S)he often engages in repetitive, stereotyped behaviors such as body rocking, hand or finger waving,
object twirling, mouthing, etc.
Yes No
24.
When (s)he engages in the behavior, you and others usually respond by doing nothing (i.e., you never or
rarely attend to the behavior.)
Yes No
25.
The behavior seems to occur in cycles. During a “high” cycle, the behavior occurs frequently and is
extremely difficult to interrupt. During a “low” cycle the behavior rarely occurs.
Yes No
26.
The behavior seems to occur more often when the person is ill. Yes No
27.
(S)he has a history of recurrent illness (e.g., ear or sinus infections, allergies, dermatitis). Yes No
Scoring Summary
Circle the items answered “Yes.” If you completed only Part II, also circle items 1, 2, and 3
Likely Maintaining Variable
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Social Reinforcement (attention)
1 2 3 9 10 11 12 13 Social Reinforcement (access to specific activities/items)
1 2 3 14 15 16 17 18 Social Reinforcement (escape)
19 20 21 22 23 24 Automatic Reinforcement (sensory stimulation)
19 20 24 25 26 27 Automatic Reinforcement (pain attenuation)
Comments/Notes: ____________________________________________________________________
__________________________________________________________________________________
__________________________________________________________________________________
__________________________________________________________________________________
66
Appendix D
Intervention Rating Profile Teacher version
The purpose of this questionnaire is to obtain information that will aid in the selection of
classroom interventions. Teachers of children with behavior problems will use these
interventions. Please circle the number which best describes your agreement or disagreement
with each statement.
Strongly
Disagree
Disagree
Slightly
Disagree
Slightly
Agree
Agree
Strongly
Agree
1. This would be an acceptable intervention
for the child’s problem behavior.
2. Most teachers would find this intervention
appropriate for behavior problems in
addition to the one described.
3. This intervention should prove effective in
changing in the child’s problem behavior.
4. I would suggest the use of this intervention
to other teachers.
5. The child’s behavior problem is severe
enough to warrant use of this intervention.
6. Most teachers would find this intervention
suitable for the behavior problem
described
7. I would be willing to use this intervention
in the classroom setting.
8. This intervention would not result in
negative side effects for the student.
9. This intervention would be appropriate for
a variety of children.
10. This intervention is consistent with those I
have used in classroom settings.
11. The intervention was a fair way to handle
the child’s problem behavior.
12. This intervention is reasonable for the
problem behavior described.
13. I like the procedures used in this
intervention.
14. This intervention was a good way to handle
this child’s behavior problem.
15. Overall, this intervention would be
beneficial for the child.
Adapted from: Witt, J. C. and Elliott, S. N. (1985). Acceptability of classroom intervention strategies. In T. R. Kratochwill (Ed.),
Advances in School Psychology, 4, 251-288. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
67
Appendix E
Institutional Review Board Approval Letter
68
VITA
ZIYAD ALRUMAYH
Education: Public Schools, Al-Qassim Province, Saudi Arabia
B.A. Special Education, Qassim University,
Al Qassim Province, Saudi Arabia 2013
M.A. Special Education, East Tennessee State
University, Johnson City, Tennessee 2018
Professional Experience: Teacher, Private School; Alrass, Saudi Arabia 2013- 2014