University of Nebraska at Omaha University of Nebraska at Omaha
DigitalCommons@UNO DigitalCommons@UNO
Student Work
5-2002
Assessing toddlers' problem-solving skills using play assessment: Assessing toddlers' problem-solving skills using play assessment:
Facilitation versus non-facilitation Facilitation versus non-facilitation
Leslie J. McCaslin
University of Nebraska at Omaha
Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/studentwork
Part of the Psychology Commons
Please take our feedback survey at: https://unomaha.az1.qualtrics.com/jfe/form/
SV_8cchtFmpDyGfBLE
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
McCaslin, Leslie J., "Assessing toddlers' problem-solving skills using play assessment: Facilitation versus
non-facilitation" (2002).
Student Work
. 291.
https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/studentwork/291
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by
DigitalCommons@UNO. It has been accepted for
inclusion in Student Work by an authorized administrator
of DigitalCommons@UNO. For more information, please
ASSESSING TODDLERS PROBLEM-SOLVING SKILLS
USING PLAY ASSESSMENT: FACILITATION VERSUS NON-FACILITATION
An Ed.S. Field Project
Presented to the
Department of Psychology
and the
Faculty o f the Graduate College
University o f Nebraska
In Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements for the Degree
Specialist in Education
University of Nebraska at Omaha
by
Leslie J. McCaslin
May, 2002
UMI Number: EP72935
All rights reserved
INFORMATION TO ALL USERS
The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed,
a note will indicate the deletion.
UMI
Dissertation Publishing
UMI EP72935
Published by ProQuest LLC (2015). Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author.
Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC.
All rights reserved. This work is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code
ProQuest LLC.
789 East Eisenhower Parkway
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, Ml 48106- 1346
EDS FIELD PROJECT ACCEPTANCE
Acceptance for the faculty o f the Graduate College,
University o f Nebraska, in partial fulfillment o f the
requirements for the degree Specialist in Education,
University o f Nebraska at Omaha.
Committee
Chairperson ^ 2 2
ASSESSING TODDLERS PROBLEM-SOLVING SKILLS
USING PLAY ASSESSMENT: FACILITATION VERSUS NON-FACILITATION
Leslie J. McCaslin, Ed.S.
University of Nebraska
Advisor: Lisa Kelly-Vance, Ph.D.
Play assessment is rapidly emerging in the field o f cognitive assessment in young
children. One aspect o f play assessment involves the identification o f the types and
levels o f problem-solving skills children possess. Information about a childs degree of
problem-solving skills could aid school psychologists in understanding the child’s level
o f cognitive development. Research in the area o f play assessment has not focused as
much attention on problem solving as it has on other components o f play. More research
is needed in order to determine if a free play session or an adult-facilitated session is
better for assessing a child’s problem-solving skills using play assessment. The purpose
o f the present study was to identify differences in problem-solving behaviors when
assessment takes place in a nonfacilitated versus a structured facilitated play assessment
session. Twenty children ages 18-48 months were observed playing in either a structured
facilitated or a nonfacilitated setting. It was expected that differences in the level o f
problem-solving behaviors would exist between the two types o f play sessions and that
certain toys would elicit more problem-solving behaviors than others. Results indicated
that there was not a significant difference in the level o f problem solving exhibited by
children in the facilitated or the nonfacilitated sessions. Considerations for future
research are discussed.
Acknowledgements
1 would like to thank my chairperson, Dr. Lisa Kelly-Vance, for her guidance and
support with my project as well as throughout my graduate career. I would also like to
thank Dr. Brigette Ryalls and Dr. Kathy Coufal for their participation as committee
members for the project.
Furthermore, I owe a special thank you to all o f the members o f the play
assessment team who helped in the process o f data collection and recruitment of
participants, especially Jane King. I was fortunate to be able to work closely with Jane,
as the nature of our projects allowed for us to collect data together. My project would not
have been successful without Janes organization and her help in recruiting participants,
scheduling data collection sessions, and keeping me focused.
Most o f all, thank you to my husband, Tom. His patience, guidance and support
throughout these past years has helped me accomplish my goals more than he will ever
know.
Table of Contents
I. Introduction/Literature Review
................................................................................
1
A. Contributions to School Psychology
...................................................................
1
B. Early Childhood Assessment
...............................................................................
2
C. Play Assessment
....................................................................................................
4
D. Problem Solving in Young Children
....................................................................
8
E. Facilitation in Play Assessment
..........................................................................
12
F. Problem Solving and Facilitation
..........................................
.
............................
15
G. Summary
........................................................................................................
16
H. The Present Study
..............................................................................
>
.................
16
II. Method
..........................................................................................................................
18
A. Participants
............................................................................................................
18
B. Setting
...................................................................................................................
19
C. Measures
..................................................................................................
.
............
20
D. Procedures
..................................
20
1. Nonfacilitated Group
.....................................................................................
21
2. Structured Facilitated Group
...............................................................
21
3. Coding
.............................................................................................................
21
4. Interrater Reliability
......................................................................................
22
5. Data Analyses
.................................................................................................
22
III. Results and Discussion
...............................................................................................
23
A. Limitations/Considerations for Future Research
..............................................
26
B. Summary
...............................................................................................................
31
IV. References
.....................................................................................................................
33
V. Appendices....
.................................
,
.........................................................
*
................
38
VI. Table..,
...........................................................................................................
41
1
Assessing Toddlers Problem-Solving Skills
Using Play Assessment: Facilitation versus Non-Facilitation
Play assessment is rapidly emerging in the field o f cognitive assessment of
preschool-aged children. Childrens play is a natural reflection o f cognitive
development. Practitioners can look toward play behaviors to gain knowledge about a
childs level o f development. One aspect of play assessment involves the identification
of the types and levels o f problem-solving skills children possess. Malone and Langone
(1999) expressed that there has not been enough attention devoted to researching
behaviors in the context o f play. Further, research in the area o f play assessment has not
tended to focus as much in the area o f problem solving as it has in other components o f
play. Within the framework o f play assessment, researchers should determine the
optimal conditions under which to assess problem-solving skills. For example, if it is
determined that some children are more apt to display problem-solving behaviors in a
structured, facilitated play session rather than in a free play session, then perhaps the
assessment should include a structured, facilitated component in order to effectively
assess the childs problem-solving skills. The purpose o f the present study was to
provide information about the type o f play setting that should be used in a play
assessment when practitioners are interested in the problem-solving component of play.
Contributions to School Psychology
One role o f the school psychologist is to provide early childhood assessment
when developmental delays are suspected in preschool-aged children. Standardized tests
are not always representative of the potential capabilities o f a young child, especially if
2
the child is disabled or disadvantaged. Play assessment could be used as an
accompaniment to traditional standardized measures of assessing cognitive development
in preschool-aged children. Observing a child’s play behaviors in a natural, non
threatening environment can provide a practitioner with information about the childs
level o f cognitive development in general as well as compared to the developmental level
of his or her peers. School psychologists working with elementary children can use
findings from preschool play assessments to help them determine the reasons children
may have been eligible for services before they started school (Ross, 2002). In particular,
information about a childs problem-solving skills is important to the school
psychologists understanding of the child’s level of cognitive development. Children
need adequate problem-solving skills to generalize problem solutions to other problems,
to gather information from several situations and experiences and use that information to
solve a new problem, and to generate alternative ways to solve a particular problem
(Chen, Sanchez, & Campbell 1997). In order to conduct a thorough assessment and
design effective interventions, the school psychologist should consider the childs
competency in solving problems once this aspect of the child’s cognitive fiinctioning is
known. The present study contributes to the field o f school psychology and early
childhood assessment by adding to the research on the specific aspect of problem solving
as it is evaluated using play assessment.
Early Childhood Assessment
Assessment can be used in the preschool years to determine if early intervention
services are needed to prevent childhood problems. This, in turn, may prevent later
3
problems (Lidz, 1977). When a parent or physician is concerned that a child is not
developing in some capacity at an appropriate rate, it is necessary for school
psychologists and other early childhood specialists to verify (or refute) these concerns
and provide assistance with early interventions to try to alleviate or diminish future
problems the child might otherwise encounter as a result o f his or her developmental
delays. The need for effective early childhood assessment has increased since the
passage o f Public Law 99-457 in 1986 that required at-risk children aged three to five to
receive assistance through the public school system. More recently, the Individuals with
Disabilities Education Act, 1997 (IDEA 97), was revised to include infants and toddlers
from birth through age two in the early education requirements.
Common instruments that are used in the United States to assess the cognitive
functioning of preschoolers are the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale: Fourth Edition (S-
B IV) (Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1986), the Differential Ability Scales (DAS) (Elliott,
1983), and the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale o f Intelligence Revised (WPPSI-
R) (Wechler, 1989). A primary purpose of such standardized tests is to assess a child’s
need for special services (Salvia & Ysseldyke, 2001).
Now that schools are focusing more on the needs o f preschool-aged children,
school psychologists must find reliable and valid methods of assessing the cognitive
functioning of these children, a task which is sometimes difficult in cases involving
young children with handicaps (Schakel 1986). Many standardized tests offer normative
data as well as strong reliability and validity measures, but often the tests cannot be
adapted to meet the needs o f exceptional children. Furthermore, standardized testing has
4
received much criticism due to its limitations. Some o f the limitations are as follows: (a)
testing generally does not occur in the childs natural setting, (b) the test results are not
appropriate for use in monitoring progress or designing interventions, and (c) the tests are
often normed on a population o f typically developing subjects, making assessment of
cognitively delayed children difficult. Criticisms o f standardized tests also focus on the
difficulty in determining whether the tests really measure the constructs they are
supposed to measure and the uncertainty about whether standardized tests are appropriate
for assessing preschool-aged children (James & Tanner, 1993). In addition, standardized
tests often lack predictive and concurrent validity, which renders them inappropriate for
assessing preschool children (Neisworth & Bagnato, 1992).
Play Assessment
Fortunately, many researchers and practitioners realize the limitations o f using
standardized testing to assess preschool-aged children and are working toward finding
more reliable and valid alternatives. One alternative currently in its infancy, although
gaining attention in the literature, is play assessment. Because play is a non-threatening
and natural activity (Lowenthal, 1997), the child is likely to exhibit behaviors during play
assessment that are typical for that child. In contrast, the child is not as likely to exhibit
typical behaviors during a standardized testing procedure in which the child is providing
responses to more structured, rigid questions or tasks with which the child is unfamiliar.
The theoretical roots of play assessment originated in the models of cognitive
development proposed by Piaget and Vygotsky. Piaget (1962) proposed a four-stage
model o f cognitive development. He distinguished among types of play that emerge
5
during the early stages. In the first stage the child forms schemas o f events that can be
later applied to new situations. Grasping, shaking or moving objects are examples of
play behaviors a child might exhibit during this stage. As the child learns to apply
existing schemas to new situations, play becomes more functional. Symbolic play
emerges during the second period, followed by more realistic symbolic play. Vygotsky
(1966) also subscribed to the stage-like notion o f play. He believed that play is a
purposeful activity and that a child develops through play, using play as a means to learn
about the environment and to eventually apply this learning to reality.
In general, play assessment involves observation o f the child’s behaviors while
playing in a naturalistic setting in order to collect information about the child’s
development and cognitive functioning across several domains (e.g., early object use and
symbolic play). The level and category o f play the child exhibits is coded. The codes are
hierarchical such that a higher play code indicates a higher level o f cognitive functioning.
In addition to several core domains such as exploratory or symbolic play, information can
be obtained about behaviors in supplemental domains, including information about the
child’s ability to problem solve. Play assessment is a broad term that describes several
measures that assess play in ways that are unique to each measure. Just as there are many
different types of standardized intelligence tests, several types o f play assessment also
exist (Athanasjou, 2000).
One type Of play assessment is the Play Assessment Scale (PAS), developed by
Fewell (Athanasioii, 2000). The PAS was designed to be used with children ages 2 ter 36
months. Each play Session consists of the child engaging in spontaneous play afid is
6
followed by a segment in which the child is prompted to play with specific toys or
respond to specific verbal and/or motor items. Another type of play assessment,
Transdisciplinary Play-Based Assessment (TPBA), developed by Linder (1993), involves
a diverse team o f people involved with several different aspects of the child’s life.
Transdisciplinary refers to the idea that a team o f people from several disciplines ate
involved in the assessment o f the child, including educators and parents. The
involvement o f parents and several disciplines in the school is an advantage over
standardized testing because people that are familiar with the child across many settings
can provide input about the childs needs. Several different aspects o f the childs
behaviors are observed as part o f TPBA. The child is observed during free play as well
as facilitated play, and interactions are observed between the child and a peer as well as
between the child and a parent. Each team member is involved in observing the child and
is subsequently involved in making educational decisions for the child.
Transdisciplinary play-based assessment formed the basis for the development of
the Play Assessment o f Cognitive Skills Scale (PACSS) (Kelly-Vance et al., 2000).
PACSS has evolved into a scale that uses a much more specific coding scheme than
Linders. The PACSS observation sessions also differ from TPBA in that observation
sessions using PACSS are limited to a free play session followed by a facilitated segment
in which the child is prompted to play with specific items he or she did not play with
while engaged in free play (Ryalls et al., 2000).
Practitioners have widely .accepted the use of play assessment as a means o f
assessing preschool-aged children (Myers, McBride, & Peterson, 1996). Unfortunately,
7
the flexibility involved in play assessment often lends itself to subjectivity in conclusions
drawn from observation, which can affect scores based on the person rating the
behaviors. Kelly-Vance, Needelman, Troia, and Ryalls (1999) found that 2-year-olds
who were assessed using a modified form of TPBA, Play-Based Assessment, and also
using the Bayley Scales o f Infant Development-II (BSID-II) scored higher on the Play-
Based Assessment than on the BSID-II. Kelly-Vance et al. noted that the children may
have been able to perform better during play because the play sessions did not involve the
restricted format of the BSID-II; however, the authors also noted that the data from the
Play-Based Assessment could have been more influenced by the rater due to the
assessment’s subjectivity.
Farmer-Dougan & Kaszuba (1999) took steps to minimize the subjectivity
involved in assessing play behaviors and to establish the reliability and validity o f play
assessment. A classroom-based play observation system was used as the play assessment
in their study, which consisted o f 42 children ages 3 to 5. The Battelle Developmental
Inventory (BDI) was used to obtain standardized scores of each child’s cognitive ability.
In addition the Social Skills Rating Scale - Teacher Form (SSRS-T) was used to measure
the childrens social skills. Play categories were defined in terms o f social play and
cognitive play. The children were videotaped playing over four 10-minute periods, and
four independent observers later coded their play behaviors. The observers coded until a
minimum interrater reliability of .90 was established. Results indicated that the
children’s play behavjprs predicted their scores on both the BDI as well ^sthe$SR $-T.
These results strengthened the credibility o f play as a viable assessment tool as long as
8
play categories are operationally defined. The present study adds to the limited amount
o f research available regarding the effectiveness o f using play assessment to measure the
cognitive development in preschool-aged children by looking specifically at children’s
problem-solving skills.
Problem Solving in Young Children
Within the area o f play assessment, a childs ability to problem solve reflects the
child’s level o f overall cognitive functioning. Research indicates that problem-solving
skills develop early in childhood. Infants as young as 6 months of age have been found
to actively elicit help from their mothers to achieve a goal (Mosier & Rogoff, 1994).
Caruso (1993) examined the exploratory and problem-solving behaviors in a group o f 11-
to 12-month-old infants. To elicit exploration, the infants were presented with toys that
were novel to the infants but not completely unfamiliar in terms of the infants prior
experience o f objects. Exploratory play was coded based on the number o f ways the
infant explored a toy, the infant’s use o f the same exploratory behavior with different
toys, and the use o f an exploratory behavior that had previously been used after using
new behaviors.
Next, problem solving was examined by using tasks specifically designed to elicit
problem solving. First, the infant was presented with a Plexiglas box that contained a
small toy. The box contained two openings, and the toy would only fit through one o f the
openings. Infants were prompted to retrieve the toy from the box. The second task
involved two Plexiglas shields placed parallel to each other and attached to a wooden
base. The shields were close enough together that an infant’s hand would not fit between
9
them. A toy was placed between the two shields with a string attached to the toy and
draped over the top and to the outside of one o f the shields. The child was again
encouraged to retrieve the toy from the apparatus. Problem-solving behaviors were then
coded according to the childs looking behaviors at both the apparatus and the toy,
behaviors directed toward the apparatus, reaching, touching, successful and unsuccessful
attempts to remove the toy, and absence of behaviors directed at the apparatus.
Information about persistence, strategy use, and sophistication in problem solving were
gathered from the coding. Problem solving was represented by the infants persistence in
trying to retrieve the toy, the number of different strategies the infant tried, and whether
the infant solved the problem right away, after some or lots o f trial and error, or not at all.
The infant’s breadth and depth o f exploratory play was then compared to the problem
solving variables to determine if relationships existed between the two types of play. The
major finding was that as early as one year o f infancy the child’s breadth o f exploratory
behaviors, or the number o f different schemes used to explore an object, were related to
the child’s problem-solving behaviors.
DeLoache, Sugarman, and Brown (1985) studied the corrections 18- to 42-month-
old children made to errors that occurred while trying to nest a set of seriated cups. The
cups were placed in front of the child and the child was told that the cups were for him or
her to play with. If after two minutes the child did not spontaneously try to nest the cups,
the experimenter fully nested the cups out of the child’s sight and then presented them to
the child. After the child could see the end result, the experimenter again took the cups
out of the child’s sight, disassembled them, and placed them back on the table. Findings
10
indicated that the childrens error correction strategies became more flexible with age,
meaning the younger children tended to focus on the fact that two o f the cups did not fit
together, while the older children incorporated strategies that involved using all o f the
cups. The authors concluded that more extensive research is needed regarding childrens
problem solving in terms of how children correct errors made while attempting to achieve
a goal.
Children not only develop strategies used to correct errors when attempting to
achieve a goal, but through this experience there seems to be a period in development
when they begin focusing on producing expected outcomes (Bullock & Lutkenhaus,
1988). Bullock and Lutkenhaus observed 15- to 35-month-old children as they
participated in play and clean-up tasks. Tasks involved using blocks to build a tower and
to dress a wooden figure. For the tower-building task, five trials were presented. Each
trial consisted of three blocks, each o f which was painted in such a way that when the
blocks were stacked into a tower they would form a picture. The children were also
presented with unpainted blocks. The experimenters were looking to see if the children
would stop building the tower once the desired outcome was reached or if they would
keep working by using the unpainted blocks. For the figure-dressing task, the children
were presented with a wooden figure that was surrounded by a box. The box contained
four blocks of different colors, and their positions in the box were marked with matching
colors painted on the inside o f the box. The children were told that the blocks were the
figures clothes and the figure needed them to stay warm. The children were also
presented with extra blocks not needed to dress the figure. After being asked to dress the
11
figure, the experimenters again looked for whether the children stopped once the desired
outcome was reached. A clean-up task involved cleaning a blackboard with chalk
scribbled on it. The children were shown how to dunk the sponge in a bucket o f water,
wring it out, and use it to clean the chalk off the board. The experimenters looked to see
whether the children would clean with the goal to get the chalk off the chalkboard and not
just move the sponge around haphazardly. Results indicated that the younger children
were more activity-oriented in that they focused on the activity in which they were
engaged rather than the outcome they were expected to produce. The older children
showed more outcome-oriented tendencies in that they stopped playing when the desired
outcome had been reached. The authors concluded that children begin to structure their
activities in relation to a desired or expected outcome around three years o f age. This is
an important finding to consider when gathering information about a preschoolers
development o f problem-solving skills. According to Bullock and Lutkenhaus, one
would expect that a 4-year-old would attempt to solve problems with outcome-oriented
goals rather than activity-oriented goals.
Research regarding young children’s development of problem-solving skills goes
beyond preschool as well. Results o f Klahr and Robinson’s (1981) study revealed that by
first grade, children have acquired a vast array of problem-solving schemes that can be
applied to novel tasks. The subjects in the study ranged from 3.6 to 6.3 years o f age. A
modified version o f the original Tower of Hanoi task (Simon, as cited in Klahr &
Robinson, 1981) was used. The tasks consisted o f three pegs, one o f which contained a
stack o f disks ranging in size. The tasks varied in goal type. The directions of one task
12
were to move the disks one at a time to a second peg, and at no time could a larger disk
be stacked on top o f a smaller disk. In a simpler version o f the task, the directions were
to make sure all the pegs were occupied by disks. The tasks also varied in difficulty,
ranging from one to seven moves required to complete the task. In order for a young
child to solve this type o f problem, the child must be able to use problem-solving skills
including systematic trial and error and planning. Results indicated that the 6-year-olds
were successful in completing the tasks involving up to six moves, but the 4-year-olds
were successful only in completing the tasks involving up to two moves. This type o f
research demonstrates that the knowledge a school psychologist gathers about a child’s
ability to problem solve will reveal information about that child’s level o f cognitive
functioning. This type o f knowledge is imperative for designing effective interventions
because the intervention must be matched with the childs ability to succeed with the
intervention.
Facilitation in Play Assessment
One aspect o f play assessments that varies among different types o f assessment is
the level o f facilitation involved in the play session. Specifically, play assessments tend
to differ with regard to the amount o f directions that are given, the toys provided, and the
ways in which behaviors are elicited from the child (Athanasiou, 2000). Facilitation is
sometimes performed, for instance, by an adult experimenter modeling behaviors for the
child (Ungerer, Zelazo, Kearsley, & OLeary, 1981; Watson & Fischer, 1977; and
Watson & Jackowitz, 1984) and sometimes by the child’s mother participating in play
with the child (Fein & Fryer, 1995).
13
Fein and Fryer (1995) were interested in finding out the effects of parental
facilitation on a child’s level and amount o f pretend play, so they reviewed research that
involved parents in the play assessments o f 12- to 36-month-old children. The authors
found that the mother’s involvement increased the amount of the childs pretense but that
results were inconclusive regarding the influence of parental involvement on the child’s
level o f sophistication in play. Watson and Jackowitz (1984) examined children’s use o f
spontaneous play by having the experimenter model talking on the phone to children ages
14 to 25 months. Then, immediately prior to leaving the room, the experimenter asked
the children to imitate the behavior while waiting for the examiner to return. Various
agents and objects were used for this task, ranging from least to most difficult in terms o f
symbolic substitutions. For example, the items ranged from the experimenter talking into
a toy telephone to a doll talking to a toy banana to a wooden block talking to a toy car, to
name a few of the steps. The children were then observed for spontaneous symbolic
play. Findings revealed that all children showed some form o f symbolic play after the
modeling occurred. Even on tasks that they performed incorrectly, they still
demonstrated some type of symbolic play. For example, children may have failed a task
in which they were asked to make the doll talk to the toy banana, but they still may have
demonstrated use of symbolic play by talking into the toy banana themselves. Similarly,
Watson and Fischer (1977) examined the effects o f modeling symbolic behaviors to
children aged 14 to 24 months. The experimenters used themselves, a doll, and a wooden
block as agents and sleeping, eating and washing as the pretend activities. These
activities were modeled to the children, and then the experimenter left the children to
14
play freely for several minutes. Findings indicated that the modeling elicited pretend
play in the majority o f the children studied. Ungerer et al. (1981) also used modeling to
examine the effects o f age on symbolic play. They studied children of 18, 22, 26, and 34
months of age. First, the children engaged in free play for several minutes. Next, the
experimenter modeled four different play behaviors before leaving the children to play
freely again. As age increased, children used more imaginative substitution in their play.
All of these studies are examples o f how facilitation has been used to study different
aspects of children’s play.
Whether facilitated or non-facilitated play assessments are better for gaining a
true representation of a child’s skills is not clear. Research regarding play assessment
involving typical children tends to involve non-facilitated play. On the other hand,
research regarding play assessment involving exceptional children often involves
facilitation (e.g., Beeghly, Weiss Perry, & Cicchetti, 1989; Roach, Stevenson, Barratt,
Miller, 8c Leavitt, 1998; Rosenburg, Robinson, & Beckman, 1986; Spencer, 1996; and
Ungerer & Sigman, 1981). Some believe that facilitators provide the child with the
necessary assistance to allow the child to demonstrate a higher level o f skills than he or
she would without facilitation during play (Linder, 1993). Others, however, believe this
is not always the case. For example, Roach et al. (1998) found that the interactions o f
mothers and their children with Down syndrome did not significantly affect the children’s
play behaviors. Some researchers use facilitation only after observing the child during
free play to encouraged the child to play with toys or perform certain tasks not observed
during free play (Linder, 1993; Ryalls et al., 2000).
15
Problem Solving and Facilitation
Whether a child’s play is facilitated or not during a play assessment could have an
impact on the developmental level that is displayed by the child during play. As
discussed earlier, facilitation has been used to study various aspects o f childrens play
(e.g., Fein & Fryer, 1995; Ungerer, Zelazo, Kearsley, & OLeary, 1981; Watson &
Fischer, 1977; Watson & Jackowitz, 1984). Whether facilitation has an impact on the
degree and amount o f problem-solving a child displays during play, however, has not
been given attention in the play assessment research. Malone, Stoneman, and Langone
(1994) suggested that play behaviors were more reflective of true developmental level in
free-play settings in which the child is allowed to play independently at home rather than
in more structured classroom settings in which the child is allowed to interact with peers.
In the free play sessions, adults were discouraged from interacting with the child as well.
Taking these findings into consideration, perhaps an adult-facilitated play setting would
hinder a child’s demonstration o f higher-order play skills than if the child were left to
play alone with no facilitation (Malone et al., 1994). Hanline (1999), while discussing
the use o f play as a learning tool, stated that in order to be effective in engaging children
in active participation in play for learning purposes, the play setting needs to be carefully
planned. This could also mean for the present study that a structured, facilitated session
would be better for engaging children in problem-solving tasks than a free-play session in
which the children may or may not engage in problem solving. While these ideas may
seem logical, the problem still exists that there is no empirical research to date that
suggests whether or not facilitation is necessary to assess a childs problem-solving skills.
16
The current study utilized the PACSS method to answer questions about whether or not
facilitation is necessary or beneficial in eliciting problem-solving behaviors in children
during play assessment.
Summary
Information about a childs problem-solving skills is an integral part of an overall
assessment of the preschool child’s cognitive development. If the level of problem
solving is to be examined as a component of play assessment, the optimal type of play
setting for inviting problem-solving behaviors must be determined. Furthermore, the
child’s skill level in problem solving without facilitation versus the child’s potential skill
level when provided with adult facilitation and prompting must be examined. The
present study examined two types of play sessions, non-facilitated versus structured
facilitated, in an attempt to determine which setting is more conducive to eliciting
problem-solving behaviors using the PACSS method.
The Present Study
The present study used PACSS to evaluate the problem-solving behaviors in
toddlers across two different types o f settings, nonfacilitated and structured facilitated.
Participants engaged in free play sessions and were divided into two groups. In the
nonfacilitated group, the participants were subject to minimal interaction with adults in
the room. In the structured facilitated group, a session facilitator adhered to structured
guidelines with respect to the toys and types o f play toward which the participants were
directed.
17
The purpose o f the present study was to determine whether the level o f problem
solving behaviors would differ in a nonfacilitated play session versus a structured
facilitated play session. No previous research has been conducted in the area of problem
solving with respect to session facilitation, and the need for research in this area has been
expressed (Kelly-Vance et al., 2000). It was expected that the results of the study would
answer the question about whether the level of problem solving behaviors displayed
throughout a play assessment would differ significantly between the two types of
sessions.
Within the structured facilitated sessions, children were asked to play with
specific toys that typically elicit problem-solving behaviors (e.g., nesting cups, blocks,
mechanical toys, and puzzles). The same toys were available to the children in the
nonfacilitated sessions, but only in the structured facilitated sessions was the children’s
attention specifically directed to those toys by an adult facilitator. Even though there is
not empirical research as of yet to link facilitation to problem-solving behaviors in play,
it was hypothesized that a higher level of problem-solving behaviors would be exhibited
during the facilitated sessions than in the nonfacilitated sessions because in the former
condition participants were specifically directed toward toys that have been demonstrated
to elicit problem-solving behaviors.
It was expected that certain types o f toys would elicit more problem-solving
behaviors than others. For example, puzzles (Carlson et al., 1998) and nesting cups
(DeLoache et al., 1985) have been demonstrated to elicit problem-solving behaviors.
Because the participants in the structured facilitated sessions were guided specifically
18
toward these types of toys, it was expected that the participants would engage in a greater
number of problem-solving behaviors in the structured facilitated setting and that those
behaviors would be more complex than in the nonfacilitated play setting.
Method
Participants
A total o f 20 typically developing children (12 boys; mean age: M = 28.00, SD =
9.18 and 8 girls; mean age: M = 27.75, SD = 10.51) participated in the study. The sample
consisted of two groups o f children who were Caucasian and from a middle-class
background as determined by maternal occupation. The groups consisted of a
nonfacilitated group and a structured facilitated group. Each group consisted o f ten 18-
to 48- month-old children. The participants were further divided into the following age
categories to be used as an initial screening for matching purposes: (a) 18-24 months, (b)
24-30 months, (c) 30-36 months, (d) 36-42 months, and (e) 42-48 months. The
participants were matched by gender as well as by standard scores as measured by the
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (see Table 1). The Vineland scores were used solely
to match participants based on their composite scores and was not used as a comparison
to their PACSS score. The Vineland measures the childs adaptive behavior skills in the
areas o f Communication, Daily Living Skills, Socialization and Motor Skills (Sparrow,
Balia, & Cicchetti, 1984). A purpose of the Vineland is to provide a norm-referenced
assessment and detailed information about a childs adaptive skills relative to other
children that child’s age (Harrison & Boan, 2000). In an attempt to control for the wide
range o f developmental abilities that surface in preschool children at varying ages, the
participants in the two groups were matched according to their Vineland composite
scores to within two standard deviations instead of being matched by chronological age.
It is important to study cognitive development in typically developing children so that
those children who have deficits in cognitive development can be easily identified as a
first step to intervention. In particular, a child’s ability to problem solve can reveal
information about the level o f cognitive development that the child has reached.
Participants were recruited through word-of-mouth. The experimenters obtained
referral lists from relatives, friends and neighbors consisting o f the contact information
for people who had children ages 18-48 months. Each parent who participated in the
study was given a referral list and was asked to provide names o f people who might also
* be interested in participating.
Setting
The sessions took place in a playroom that was used for play assessment research
at the University o f Nebraska at Omaha. The room consisted of a variety o f toys that
have been shown to elicit various types o f play. Included in the toy selection, but not
limited to these items, was a kitchen set with dishes and pretend food; dolls and related
toys such as a high chair, stroller, blanket, and bottles; a doctor’s bag and veterinary kit; a
tool bench with plastic tools; mechanical toys such as a pretend gumball machine and
pop-up toy; trucks and cars; a barnyard set; play telephones; and blocks and puzzles,
which tend to elicit problem-solving strategies (Carlson, Taylor, & Levin, 1998). Present
in the playroom during each session was a camera operator, a session facilitator, and a
parent/guardian o f the child.
20
Measures
A portion o f the PACSS coding scheme was used and is presented in Appendix A
(Kelly-Vance et al., 2000). The PACSS coding scheme is intended to operationalize
cognitive development in toddlers in the area o f problem solving. The coding scheme
was selected because of its established use in prior related research (Kelly-Vance et al.,
2000) examining play assessment.
Behaviors sampled by the PACSS coding scheme include those codes listed in the
problem solving and planning subdomain of the coding scheme (see Appendix A). The
overall coding scheme encompasses several aspects o f play including exploratory and
symbolic play as well as several subdomains including problem solving and planning,
categorization, and imitation. The present study is part of a larger study comparing the
overall effects of facilitation on childrens play, which utilizes all o f the core domains o f
the PACSS coding scheme. O f specific interest to the present study was the problem
solving and planning subdomain. Thus, for the present study, the problem solving and
planning subdomain is the only category from the coding scheme that is addressed.
Procedures
An experimenter interviewed one parent o f each of the participants using the
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales to determine an Adaptive Behavior Composite score
for each participant. The interview was conducted within one week of each session. In
addition, during each session the parent was given a consent form to read, sign and date
and was asked to fill out a demographics questionnaire, a checklist of toys the child had
at home, and a referral list.
21
Nonfacilitated group. In the nonfacilitated group, the children were allowed to
play freely for the entire session with minimal interaction with adults. No specific
guidelines were set with regards to the type o f play in which the child was allowed to
engage or the specific toys with which the child was allowed to play. Present in each
session was a session facilitator, whose main role was to answer parent questions; a
camera operator; and a parent/caregiver. Adults were instructed not to guide the child’s
play. General statements that adults were allowed to communicate to the child during the
nonfacilitated sessions were posted on the wall. These statements consisted mostly o f
one- to two-word phrases (e.g., “wow!, “good job”) and instructions (e.g., smile”, y ° u
can imitate) and are not thought to facilitate play behaviors in the child.
Structured facilitated group. In the structured facilitated group, the conditions
were the same as for the nonfacilitated group except that the facilitator initiated play with
the participants by following a structured set o f guidelines (Appendix B). The facilitator
made a maximum of two attempts at facilitating the child toward a particular activity or
toy. If the child did not demonstrate interest after the two attempts, the facilitator moved
on to another activity or toy from the list of guidelines.
Coding. Each play session was videotaped and lasted a minimum o f 30 minutes.
Videotapes were then observed and problem-solving behaviors were coded by a PACSS
team member. The codes are hierarchical from the least to the highest level of problem
solving. The highest code observed during 30 minutes of play was recorded for each
child.
22
Interrater Reliability. Interrater reliability was established through extensive
training and was maintained at a level o f .90 or greater by calculating the reliability
between two independent observers for all o f the play sessions coded. To become
proficient in using the PACSS coding scheme, the experimenters were trained by coding
videotaped play sessions obtained from a separate play assessment study. Codes were
assigned for every 30-second interval o f play, and a group o f play assessment team
members discussed the codes and any discrepancies among the team members until
overall reliability o f .90 was established for the group. In the current study sessions were
coded simultaneously by two observers. One of the observers took descriptive notes of
the session, including the amount of time spent in certain types of play. At the same
time, a second observer took informal notes about the child’s activities. At the end of
each session, the observers separately recorded the highest level o f play from the core
subdomains as well as the highest level o f problem solving observed during the 30-
minute session, and the two observers checked for agreement. Overall reliability is
determined by dividing the total number of agreements by the total number o f agreements
plus disagreements, then obtaining a percentage. Interrater reliability was maintained at a
level o f 100% both overall and specifically for problem solving.
Data Analyses. Two analyses were conducted. A quantitative analysis consisted
o f obtaining codes for each play session from the Problem Solving and Planning
subdomain. From these codes, the highest level o f problem solving behavior displayed in
each 30- minute session was determined. O f specific interest were the highest level o f
problem solving and the types of toys that elicited the problem-solving behaviors. For
23
the first analysis, the independent variable was the type of session (nonfacilitated versus
structured facilitated). The dependent variable was the level o f problem-solving
behaviors. A one-way analysis o f variance was conducted to compare the highest level
of problem solving in the facilitated group with the highest level o f problem solving in
the nonfacilitated group. The second analysis was qualitative in nature and provides
descriptive data regarding toy type.
Results and Discussion
The highest level of problem solving was coded for each 30-minute play session.
On average, the highest level o f problem solving for the facilitated group (M = 9.20, SD
= 1.87) was comparable to that of the nonfacilitated group (M = 9.00, SD = 0.94), and a
one-way analysis of variance confirmed that the differences were nonsignificant, F(l,18)
= 0.09.
The second analysis was qualitative in nature and provides descriptive data
regarding toys that elicited problem-solving behaviors. Participants in the facilitated
group were specifically directed toward, but not restricted to, the toys and activities listed
in Appendix B. Of those toys, problem-solving behaviors as defined by the PACSS
scheme were elicited by puzzles, a gumball machine, a Disney pop-up toy, nesting cups,
shape sorters, blocks, and Velcro food from the kitchen area. The only toy included in
the facilitated guidelines that did not appear to elicit problem-solving behaviors in either
session type was the bucket of bears. Further, of the toys used to facilitate the
participants in the facilitated group, all o f those that elicited problem solving in the
facilitated group also elicited problem solving in the nonfacilitated group except for the
24
blocks. This does not mean that children did not play with the blocks; however, it simply
means that they did not problem solve or plan with the blocks. Other toys in the
playroom that elicited problem-solving behaviors for both groups included a pop-up toy,
a vase of plastic flowers, a train set, a tool set, and baby bottles. Most o f the problem
solving behaviors included either systematic or nonsystematic trial-and-error problem
solving with these toys, although the children who placed the flowers in a vase received
higher-level codes for being able to put objects into small openings. In addition, the pop
up toy and the gumball machine elicited higher codes than trial-and-error problem
solving for the children who were able to successfully operate the toys on the first try.
Children would often turn puzzle pieces and try them in different positions until the
pieces fit. The train track easily came apart, and children would try putting different
pieces of the track together to reassemble it.
It was expected that a higher level of problem-solving behaviors would be seen
during the facilitated sessions than the nonfacilitated sessions because of the facilitators
direction toward specific toys that were believed to elicit problem solving. However, this
was not the case. Participants tended to play with the toys in which they were interested.
Appendix C illustrates which toys elicited the highest levels o f problem-solving
behaviors within each session type. There are some differences, as would be expected
due to individual differences within each group, but overall the two groups did not differ
greatly in their selection o f toys. One interesting observation is that puzzles elicited the
most instances of problem solving o f any o f the toys, and the majority o f these instances
occurred in the facilitated group.
25
The question of whether facilitation has an impact on the degree o f problem
solving a child displays during play has not previously been given attention in the play
assessment research. In the current study, free play sessions involved adults who were
discouraged from interacting with the child to the extent that the child’s play would be
guided or facilitated. Malone, Stoneman, and Langone (1994) suggested that play
behaviors are more reflective of true developmental level in free-play settings in which
the child is allowed to play independently at home rather than in more structured
classroom settings in which the child is allowed to interact with peers. Although these
authors were not referring to play assessment, their findings still apply. According to
those findings, it would be expected that structured facilitated sessions would hinder a
childs problem-solving behaviors. This was not necessarily the case because problem
solving behaviors did not differ between the two types o f settings. According to these
results, facilitation did not help nor hinder problem solving.
In contrast to the views of Malone et al. (1994), Hanline (1999) stated that in
order to effectively engage children in active participation in play for learning purposes,
the play setting needs to be carefully planned. Again, the author was. not referring
specifically to play assessment as it was used in the current study; however, the idea that
play needs to be structured in order to engage children to participate applies directly to
the reasoning behind examining facilitation in play assessment as part o f the current
study. However, the play assessment used in the current study was not set up for the
child’s learning purposes, and the type of facilitation used in the facilitated sessions
might not coincide with what Hanline (1999) would consider carefully planned. To
26
date there is no empirical research that suggests whether or not facilitation is necessary to
assess a childs problem-solving skills in the form of play assessment. Results of the
present study provided a foundation for future research to answer this question; in this
study, problem solving was not significantly affected by session type.
Limitations and Considerations for Future Research
Results revealed that problem-solving behaviors exhibited during play
assessments did not differ significantly with respect to session type when the sessions
examined were purely non-facilitated versus structured facilitated. Toys that elicited
problem solving also did not differ greatly between the two types o f sessions (see
Appendix C).
A possible limitation o f the study is that because o f the small sample size,
generalizability o f the findings is limited. It was decided that a small sample would be
selected due to the exploratory nature o f the study. A wider range of problem-solving
behaviors might be found in a larger sample size. Future research should include larger
samples.
Another possible limitation concerns the internal validity of the study. It is
possible that the two types o f play sessions being compared did not differ enough to be
certain that any differences found in problem-solving behaviors can be attributed to the
type o f play session. In fact, since no significant differences were found, it is possible
that the construct o f problem solving was not sufficiently tapped in either session type.
Future research should include comparisons between several different types of play
sessions.
27
A third possible limitation concerns the number of opportunities available for
problem solving in each session. Perhaps future research could include longer play
sessions, allowing more time for children to engage in problem-solving behaviors.
Future research should also address situations in which specific problem-solving tasks
have been set up and requests made o f the child to problem solve. For example, one o f
the toys included in the present study was a train set. Although unintended by the
examiners, the train as well as the train track easily came apart while being played with.
As a result, children who wanted to continue playing with the train set were forced to
problem solve to put the set back together. This illustrates one type o f task that could be
included to facilitate problem solving. Other ideas should be explored in future research.
Problem solving is sometimes difficult to define in terms of a childs behaviors
and whether or not the childs actions actually constitute problem solving or some other
type o f cognition. For example, a childs temperament could have more to do with his or
her apparent ability to solve a problem than actual cognitive ability. The child could have
the cognitive skills available to solve a challenging problem but perhaps a low tolerance
for frustration or a tendency to give up easily, which could limit his or her success in
solving the problem. Due to the subjective nature o f the phenomenon, finding objective
means of ranking problem-solving behaviors from least to most sophisticated is difficult.
More research in the area o f problem solving is vital in this aspect. Without a great deal
o f empirical evidence regarding problem solving and play assessment, researchers and
practitioners should interpret a childs level o f problem solving with caution when using
a hierarchical coding system for problem-solving behaviors.
28
Some observations were made throughout the process of collecting data for the
present study regarding the PACSS coding scheme (see Appendix A) and some ways in
which it might be revised to diminish the amount of subjectivity in some of the codes.
For example, the first three levels o f problem solving were never used in the current
study and should be given careful consideration, if not completely omitted, in future
research. The code Searches for an object after seeing it disappearwould not be
appropriate in an assessment unless the assessment protocol called for the facilitator to
purposefully hide an object. The code Repeats behavior in order to repeat an initially
accidental consequenceis highly subjective because o f the difficulty in determining
whether a consequence was accidental. Likewise, the code “Performs a behavior in order
to produce an anticipated resultis highly subjective due to the difficulty in determining
if the child was anticipating a result.
Another consideration is that the use o f the term “achieve goal in two of the
codes in the hierarchy should be more clearly defined, again due to its subjectivity.
Unless a child specifically states his or her intentions, it is often difficult to determine the
reasons for the child’s behaviors. If a child achieves an obvious goal, then the problem
solving behaviors will probably be easily noticed; however, if the child does not achieve
a goal and that goal was not obvious to the coders, the child’s attempts at achieving that
goal could easily go unrecognized as problem solving.
Some o f the codes in the hierarchy are not especially subjective, but whether they
represent true problem-solving ability and are truly hierarchical should be further
explored. For example, a child who Successfully operates a mechanical toy on the first
29
attempt and attempts thereafterwould receive a higher level o f problem solving than a
child who does not successfully operate the toy on the first attempt but uses systematic
trial-and-error problem solving in an attempt to operate the toy. The child who tries
several different methods until he or she successfully operates the toy is clearly problem
solving, but the child who is able to operate the toy on the first try has not solved a ,
problem. Likewise, putting small objects into small openings probably requires good fine
motor skills, but if a child is able to put a small object into a small opening with no
problem, it seems unlikely that the child is exhibiting problem-solving behaviors. Most
likely, the child has problem solved in the past in order to be able to put small objects
into small openings, but once the skill is mastered, the problem no longer exists.
It could be that the codes are measuring too narrow o f a construct. For example, a
child who exhibits nonsystematic trial-and-error problem solving simply gets a lower-
level code than a child who exhibits systematic trial-and-error problem solving.
However, the child who exhibits systematic trial-and-error problem-solving might give
up a lot easier and never solve the problem, while the nonsystematic trial-and-error
problem solver might demonstrate persistence in trying to solve the problem. A child
who knows how to problem solve but lacks persistency might not function as well as a
child who has less-developed problem-solving skills but is persistent when faced with a
problem. Hupp and Abbeduto (1991) studied persistence in young children with
developmental delays. They hypothesized that children who demonstrate persistence in
solving a particular problem are also demonstrating motivation to achieve a goal. The
authors found that persistence was a reflection o f mastery motivation and posited that
30
children’s mastery behavior, or persistence, is important in helping them to learn about
their environment. Likewise, the child who “Uses an adult to achieve a goalreceives a
much lower code than the child who exhibits nonsystematic trial-and-error problem
solving, but what about the child who first uses nonsystematic trial-and-error and, upon
failure to solve the problem, asks an adult for help? It seems that this child is capable of
trying more than one approach to solve the problem, but he or she only receives credit for
nonsystematic trial-and-error problem solving.
Finally, a code that was rarely used in this study was “Uses blocks to build
complex structure [of nine or more pieces]. Again, it is difficult to determine exactly
what constitutes problem solving with this code. The subdomain includes planning as
well as problem solving, and a child who builds a complex structure has probably used
some planning skills; however, this seems difficult to determine objectively. A child
could easily use nine blocks to build a structure that was not planned. Again, the
question arises as to whether this constitutes problem solving in the same sense as the
other codes in the hierarchy. For example, a child who completes a complex, non-inset
puzzle using systematic trial-and-error problem solving would receive a lower level code
than a child who puts nine blocks together to make a wall.
The hierarchical nature o f the codes in the problem-solving and planning
subdomain was not supported by the current study, as is evident by the previously
mentioned limitations involving the problem-solving codes. This is not surprising
considering that the coding scheme, as developed by Linder (1993), has little empirical
support for its hierarchy. The codes were established from one study, which was limited
31
in sample size and heterogeneity o f participants. In fact, the participants used in the
study had hearing impairments, which limits the generalizability o f the coding scheme to
other populations. If hierarchical codes are going to be used when assessing problem
solving skills in play assessments, further research is needed to determine a more
concrete hierarchy o f problem-solving skills. Practitioners and researchers should also
consider whether a code level is even necessary. A detailed description o f the child’s
problem-solving behaviors and strategies may be more valuable in evaluating a child’s
skills and designing interventions than a standard score. Further, practitioners and
researchers must consider the generalizability of the problem-solving skills elicited
during play assessment to the types o f problems encountered in everyday life and in the
classroom. Although the coding scheme did not prove to be an objective measure of
complexity in problem-solving skills, a hierarchical measure o f problem solving may not
be necessary in play assessment.
Summary
The purpose of the present study was to gain information about whether problem
solving skills would be better assessed in a structured facilitated play session or in a
nonfacilitated play session. Research in the area o f problem solving and play assessment
is scarce, yet play assessment in general is gaining popularity in the field of early
childhood assessment. Although the sample size was small and homogeneous with
regard to ethnicity and socioeconomic status, results indicate that facilitation did not
significantly affect the level of problem solving exhibited by the participants. As play
assessment becomes more widely used, it is important for practitioners to know what type
32
o f play assessment will yield results that are the most reflective o f the child’s abilities and
skills.
/
33
References
Athanasiou, M. S. (2000). Play-based approaches to preschool assessment. In Bracken, B.
A. (Eds.), The psychoeducational assessment o f preschool children, (pp. 412-
427). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Beeghly, M., Weiss Perry, B., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Structural and affective dimensions
of play development in young children with Down syndrome. International
Journal o f Behavioral Development, 12, 257-277.
Bullock, M., & Lutkenhaus, P. (1988). The development of volitional behavior in the
toddler years. Child Development, 59, 664-674.
Carlson, S. M., Taylor, M., & Levin, G. R. (1998). The influences o f culture on pretend
play: The case o f Mennonite children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 538-565.
Caruso, D. A. (1993). Dimensions of quality in infants exploratory behavior:
Relationships to problem-solving ability. Infant Behavior and Development, 16,
441-454.
Chen, Z., Sanchez, R. P., & Campbell, T. (1997). From beyond to within their grasp: The
rudiments o f analogical problem solving in 10- and 13-month-olds.
Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 790-801.
DeLoache, J. S., Sugarman, S., & Brown, A. L. (1985). The development of error
correction strategies in young childrens manipulative play. Child Development,
56, 928-939.
Elliot, C. D. (1990). Differential Ability Scales: Introductory and technical handbook.
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
34
Farmer-Dougan, V., & Kaszuba, T. (1999). Reliability and validity o f play-based
observations: Relationship between the play behaviour observation system and
standardized measures o f cognitive and social skills. Educational Psychology,
19(4), 429-442.
Fein, G. 8c Fryer, M. (1995). Maternal contributions to early symbolic play competence.
Developmental Review, 15, 367-381.
Hanline, M. F. (1999). Developing a preschool play-based curriculum. International
Journal o f Disability, Development and Education, 46(3), 289-305.
Harrison, P. L., & Boan, C. H. (2000). Assessment o f adaptive behavior. In Bracken, B.
A. (Eds.), The psychoeducational assessment o f preschool children, (pp. 124-
144). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Hupp, S. C., & Abbeduto, L. (1991). Persistence as an indicator o f mastery motivation in
young children with cognitive delays. Journal o f Early Intervention, 15(3), 219-
225.
James, J. C., & Tanner, C. K. (1993). Standardized testing o f young children. Journal o f
Research and Development in Education, 26(3), 143-152.
Kelly-Vance, L., Gill, K., Schoneboom, N., Chemey, I., Ryan, C., Cunningham, J., &
Ryalls, B. (March, 2000). Coding play assessment: Issues, challenges, and
recommendations. Poster presented at the annual meeting o f the National
Association o f School Psychologists. New Orleans, LA.
Kelly-Vance, L., Needelman, H., Troia, K., & Ryalls, B. O. (1999). Early childhood
assessment: A comparison o f the Bayley Scales o f Infant Development and play
35
assessment in two-year-old at-risk children. Developmental Disabilities Bulletin,
27(1), 1-15.
Klahr, D., Sc Robinson, M. (1981). Formal assessment o f problem-solving and planning
processes in preschool children. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 113-148.
Lidz, C. S. (1977). Issues in the psychological assessment o f preschool children. Journal
o f School Psychology, 15(2), 129-135.
Linder, T. W. (1993). Transdisciplinary play assessment. (2nd ed.) Baltimore: Paul H.
Brookes.
Lowenthal, B. (1997). Useful early childhood assessment: Play-based, interviews and
multiple intelligences. Early Child Development and Care, 129, 43-49.
Malone, D. M., Sc Langone, F. (1999). Teaching object-related play skills to preschool
children with developmental concerns. International Journal o f Disability,
Development and Education, 46(3), 325-336.
Malone, D. M., Stoneman, Z., Sc Langone, J. (1994). Contextual variation of
correspondences among measures of play and developmental level of preschool
children. Journal o f Early Intervention, 18(2), 199-215.
Mosier, C., Sc Rogoff, B. (1994). Infants instrumental use o f their mothers to achieve
their goals. Child Development, 65, 70-79.
Myers, C. L., McBride, S. L., Sc Peterson, C. A. (1996). Transdisciplinary, play
assessment in early childhood special education: An examination o f social
validity.
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 16,
102-126.
36
Neisworth, J. T., & Bagnato, S. J. (1992). The case against intelligence testing in early
intervention. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 12(1), 1-20.
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.
Rettig, M. (1998). Environmental influences on the play o f young children with
disabilities. Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental
Disabilities, 33,189-194.
Roach, M. A., Stevenson Barratt, M., Miller, J. F., & Leavitt, L. A. (1998). The structure
o f mother-child play: Young children with Down syndrome and typically
developing children. Developmental Psychology, 34, 77-87.
Rosenburg, S. A., Robinson, C. C., & Beckman, P. J. (1986). Measures o f parent-infant
interaction: An overview. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 6, 32-43.
Ross, R. P. (2002). Best practices in the use o f play for assessment and intervention with
young children. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.). Best Practices in School
Psychology IV, Volume 2.
Ryalls, B., Gill, K., Ruane, A., Chemey, I., Schoneboom, N., Cunningham, J., and Ryan,
C. (2000). Conducting valid and reliable play-based assessments. Poster
presented at the annual meeting of the National Association o f School
Psychologists. New Orleans, LA.
Salvia, J., & Ysseldyke, J. E. (2001). Assessment o f students. In P. Coryell (Ed.),
Assessment (pp. 4-22). Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company.
Schakel, J. A. (1986). Cognitive assessment o f preschool children. School Psychology
Review,15(2), 200-215.
37
Sparrow, S. S., Balia, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior
Scales. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.
Spencer, P. E. (1996). The association between language and symbolic play at two years:
Evidence from deaf toddlers. Child Development, 67, 867-876.
Thorndike, R. M., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Stanford-Binet Intelligence
Scale, Fourth Edition. Chicago: Riverside.
Ungerer, J. A. & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in
autistic children. American Academy o f Child Psychiatry, 20, 318-337.
Ungerer, J. A., Zelazo, P. R., Kearsley, R. B., & OLeary, K. (1981). Developmental
changes in the representation o f objects in symbolic play from 18 to 34 months of
age. Child Development, 52, 186-195.
Watson, M. W. & Fischer, K. W. (1977). A developmental sequence of agent use in late
infancy. Child Development, 48, 828-836.
Watson, M. W. & Jackowitz, E. R. (1984). Agents and recipient objects in the
development o f early symbolic play. Child Development, 55, 1091-1097.
Wechsler, D. (1989). Manual fo r the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale o f
IntelligenceRevised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1966). Play and its role in the mental development o f the child. Soviet
Psychology, 12(6), 62-76.
38
Appendix A
Problem-Solving Skills and Planning
1. Searches for an object after seeing it disappear
2. Repeats behavior in order to repeat an initially accidental consequence
3. Performs a behavior in order to produce an anticipated result
4. Attempts to use an adult to achieve a goal (with or without success)
5. Makes a single attempt to activate mechanical toy or achieve goal,
unsuccessfully
6. Uses nonsystematic trial-and-error problem-solving without systematically
changing behavior
7. Uses an object or toy to obtain an object
8. Uses systematic trial-and-error problem-solving (e.g., alters behavior in an
attempt to solve problems)
9. Successfully operates a mechanical toy on first attempt and attempts thereafter
(e.g., gumball machine, Disney pop-up toy)
10. Puts small objects into little openings (the size o f a golf ball or smaller)
11. Solves problems by logically relating one experience to another (child states
that present situation is like a previously experienced situation)
12. Uses blocks to build complex structure (minimum o f nine pieces or a structure
that can easily be identified)
39
Appendix B
Facilitation Guidelines
Encourage the child to play with the specific toys contained in the following toy list by
saying, Here, let's play with these.
If they do not play with the toys, say,
What can you do with this toy?
Toy List
Nesting cups
Bears
Blocks
Puzzles
Shape sorter
Gumball machine or Cash register (child must play with one)
Drawing
When you are playing with the bears and/or blocks, give the following specific
commands:
Hand me th e
_________________
one.
Big
Little
Tall
Short
Tallest
Shortest
First, middle, last (you will have to line up 3 bears)
Go to the kitchen area and say:
L e ts make dinner.
During this time you may say:
What are you doing? and “What else can you do?
40
Appendix C
Highest Level of Problem-Solving Behaviors Elicited by Each Toy for Each Session
Type:
Session Type
Facilitated Nonfacilitated
Highest
Level
# of Sessions
Highest Level
Demonstrated
Highest
Level
# of Sessions
Highest Level
Demonstrated
Toys: Airplane
- -
8
1
Blocks*
12
1 -
-
Bottles
6
1
6
1
Car and people
6
1
-
-
Carwash
6
1 -
-
Cash register*
-
-
4 1
Comb
-
-
10
1
Farm set
- -
8
1
Flowers in vase
10 3
10
2
Gumball machine*
9 2
9
2
House
6
1
-
-
Legos
12 1
-
-
Nesting cups*
8
2
8
3
Pop-up toy
8
1
9
3
Puzzles*
8
6
8
2
Shape sorter*
8
1
8
2
Tool set
10
2
8
1
Train set
4
1
8
3
Velcro food*
8
3
-
-
Note: Toys listed in the Facilitation Guidelines (Appendix B) are denoted with an asterisk (*).
41
Table 1
Participant Age. Gender, and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Composite (ABC) Score
Participant
Code
Age (months)
Gender
Vineland
ABC Score
IN 32
F
111 ±4
IF
29 F 84 ±4
2N 22 F
85 ±5
2F
18
F
112 ±5
3N 26 M 102 ±4
3F
27 M
99 ±4
4N
18 F
112 ± 5
4F
18 F 109 ±5
5N
30 M
101 ±4
5F
30 M
104 ±4
6N
44
M 90 ±5
6F
48 M 104 ±5
7N
19
M
104 ± 5
7F
19
M
110 ± 5
8N
43 F 94 ±5
8F
42
F 107 ±5
9N
25 M
104 ± 4
9F
24
M
110 ± 4
ION
22 M 98 ±5
10F
22
M 87 ±5
Note. In the participant codes, F = Facilitated, N = Nonfacilitated.