Claremont Colleges
Scholarship @ Claremont
*$))+ '$(*1 + + *$))+,- ',#(%*+#$)

Early Childhood iPad Use and Eects on Visual
Spatial Aention Span
Maya Espiritu
Scripps College
1$+) ' ++ '$(*1 +$+$+*(-"#,,(.(-!(*!* '() ' ++.,# *$))+,- ',#(%*+#$),#(%*+#$)%* &(',,#+ '
 ), !(*$'%-+$('$'*$))+ '$(*1 + +.'-,#(*$/ &$'$+,*,(*(!#(%*+#$)%* &(',(*&(* $'!(*&,$(')% + (',,
+#(%*+#$)-%* &(', -
 (&& ' $,,$('
+)$*$,-.*%.#$%#(($+ '0 ,+('$+-%),$%2 ',$(')' Scripps Senior eses. ) *
#2)+#(%*+#$)%* &(', -+*$))+,# + +
! !
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
EARLY CHILDHOOD IPAD USE
AND EFFECTS ON VISUAL SPATIAL ATTENTION SPAN
by
MAYA ESPIRITU
SUBMITTED TO SCRIPPS COLLEGE IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT
OF THE DEGREE OF BACHELOR OF ARTS
PROFESSOR LEMASTER
PROFESSOR SPEZIO
DECEMBER 11, 2015
!
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
2!
Abstract
Despite the rising prevalence of mobile media in young children’s lives, little research exists that
examines the effects of mobile media use on early childhood cognitive development. This study
will explore how mobile media use, specifically iPad use, in early childhood affects development
of visual spatial attention span. Researchers will recruit 160 participants, ages 3 to 6, and
categorize them into three groups: TV viewers only, interactive iPad users, and passive iPad
users. Children will complete a computer task to measure the length of their visual spatial
attention span. Parents will report on the average daily amount of media use, their child’s top
three most viewed or used programs and apps, and the pervasiveness of media use, as well as
complete a demographics survey. Research assistants will rate the level of exogenous stimuli
children are exposed to in their top three programs and apps. Researchers predict that iPad use
will be associated with shorter visual spatial attention spans in comparison to TV viewing, due to
longer amounts of use, higher levels of exogenous stimuli, and higher levels of pervasiveness. In
addition, researchers hypothesize that interactive iPad use will correlate with the shortest visual
spatial attention spans due to highest levels of exogenous stimuli and longest amounts of use.
The results will help parents and educators to more effectively monitor young children’s mobile
media usage. !!
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
3!
Infants on iPads? Toddlers on touchscreens? Introducing the new, instant quick-fix to
backseat bickering and rowdy restaurant outings: mobile media. Since the release of the first
iPad in April 2010, the rate of children with personal access to mobile media has accelerated
rapidly. From 2011 to 2013, the percentage of tablet-owning families with children age 8 and
younger jumped five-fold from 8% to 40% (Common Sense Media, 2013). Because of its
portability and easy-to-use features, mobile media has become ubiquitous in many children’s
lives. Many parents are using it to occupy young children during daily routines such as running
errands, riding in a car, and eating out, turning it into a common behavioral regulation tool
(Radesky, Schumacher,& Zuckerman, 2015). From 2011 to 2013, the percentage of children 8
and under who have used mobile media for media activity or media viewing has increased from
38% to 72% (Common Sense Media, 2013). Parents have also introduced mobile media devices
to toddlers. In 2013, 38% of children under 2 had used a mobile media device compared to 10%
in 2011 (Common Sense Media, 2013). Within the same time frame, 2011 to 2013, tablet
ownership has also increased among low-income families, from 2% to 20%; and the percentage
of lower-income children who have used a mobile media device has tripled from 22% to 65%.
Overall, today’s toddlers and young children have more access to more kinds of screens
than ever before (Anderson & Pempek, 2005). However, despite this increasing prevalence in
mobile media use among children, very little research exists on how interactive mobile media
affect child development. Current research on effects of screen time on child development has
focused on the effects of TV, even when TV viewing among children has decreased and mobile
media use has increased (Common Sense Media, 2013). In light of this trend, it is imperative to
research how early childhood use of mobile media affects development. This proposed study
focuses on the amount, pervasiveness, and content of early childhood mobile media iPad use
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
4!
from ages 3 through 6 and its effects on the development of sustained visual spatial attention
span. In this study, the average daily media use signifies the amount of media use, the variety of
locations of use indicates the pervasiveness of use, and the level of exogenous stimuli denotes
the content of use. Researchers define visual spatial attention span as the maintenance of
attention over time to specific stimuli in a visual environment.!
Attention Development!
Because development of an attention span is crucial for early development, it is important
to research the ways in which mobile media use may affect attention span. Young children’s
capacity to learn about the visual world is dependent on their ability to focus, prioritize, and
control their attention (Kannass, Oakes, & Shaddy, 2006). Due to their undeveloped linguistic
and motor abilities, young children rely on attention to manage the selection and gathering of
information needed in order to learn about the world. Attention theory, specifically regarding the
development of typical attention span, will shed light on how interactive mobile media may
influence attention span development. This proposed study will focus specifically on visual
spatial attention span.!
Visual spatial attention span orientation can be either endogenous or exogenous (Chica,
Bartolomeo, & Lupiáñez, 2013). Endogenous orienting of attention is top-down, or voluntary
attention. Spatial attention is oriented endogenously when the observer chooses to orient his or
her attention to specific, relevant stimuli, usually by the interpretation of a cue that directs
attention to the target. In contrast, exogenous orienting of attention is bottom-up, or involuntary,
stimulus-driven attention. Exogenous stimuli comprise multimodal salient stimuli external to the
observer such as a moving or flashing target. Salient stimuli can exogenously capture the
observer’s attention even when he or she has not intended to direct attention to that target.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
5!
Michael Posner’s spatial cueing paradigm illustrates endogenous and exogenous orienting of
attention (Posner, 1980). In the paradigm, attention can be oriented either endogenously, using a
spatially predictive central cue, or exogenously, using a spatially non-predictive peripheral cue,
in order to detect a presented target. Figure 1 presents a model of the exogenous and endogenous
cues used in the Posner paradigm. In this cueing paradigm, observers’ ability to focus on the task
and detect the target is significantly better when the target is presented at the cued location using
endogenous cueing. Thus, the ability to intentionally and purposefully focus one’s attention is
associated with better awareness and focus.
Figure 1. Posner special cueing paradigm cues
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
As for the development of attention, in infancy, exogenous stimuli initially controls
attention. However, as children develop and gain experience, endogenous stimuli come to
influence their attention (Ruff & Capozzoli, 2003; Oakes, Kannass, & Shaddy, 2002; Wright et
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
6!
al., 1984). Researchers have developed the exploration-to-search model to explain this sequence
of attention development (Wright & Vlietstra, 1975; Huston & Wright, 1983). In the initial
exploration stage, external, perceptually salient stimuli govern young children’s attention. As
children explore their environment, their ability to focus their attention is dependent mainly on
novel, external stimuli that can capture and hold their attention. Stimuli high in perceptual
salience that often attract and hold children’s attention in early development include stimuli with
novel movements, contrasts, surprises, complexity, and incongruity (Berlyne, 1958 as cited in
Gola & Calvert, 2011). Mobile media, such as iPads, with their numerous entertaining and
interactive apps and games, constitute perceptually salient, exogenous stimuli that are enticing
and engaging to infants and young children. As a consequence, young children can intently focus
on mobile media activities for long periods of time. However, existing research has not explored
whether sustained visual spatial attention can be transferred from exogenous driven mobile
media to more endogenous driven materials. This study proposes to examine the effect of mobile
media use on attention, to determine if endogenous driven attention is deterred when children are
continuously exposed to exogenous stimuli through frequent iPad use.
Endogenous attention develops within the “search” stage of the exploration and search
model. In this stage, children transition to an active and selective search for information and
engaging stimuli. They make a conscious decision to maintain their attention to specific stimuli
regardless of its novelty or perceptual salience (Wright & Vlietstra, 1975; Huston & Wright,
1983). Ruff and Capozzoli (2003) address these two stages of attention as two attention systems:
the first is highly influenced by novelty of objects and the second is mainly self-generated. They
found that the transition to the second, higher-level system of attention occurs around 12 months
of age when children begin to habituate more quickly to novelty. From 12 months onward, the
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
7!
duration of attention based on the first system should decline as the second system of attention
slowly develops, improving cognitive skills and increasing self-regulatory skills. The second
system begins to strongly govern attention in the later preschool years.
Ruff and Capozzoli (2003) also found an effect on the type of attention based on age. In a
study of 172 children at ages 10, 26, and 42 months old, researchers coded attention during play
with toys as casual, settled, or focused. Overall, their results indicate a developmental transition
in the processes underlying attention during children’s play, such that casual attention decreased
while focused attention increased with age. Researchers found that even when the youngest, 10-
month-olds, were focused, they were more easily distracted than the older children. The oldest,
42-month-olds, were more focused, even in the presence of distracters. Kannass, Oakes, and
Shaddy (2006) corroborate this research. They found that at 31 months of age, children who are
effective at ignoring distracters are better able to maintain attention for longer durations during
multiple object free play. The results also indicate that typically developing preschoolers across
development are more effective than preschoolers with attention deficits at maintaining sustained
attention in tasks. Therefore, at around 3 years of age, typical development should show children
possessing more focused, sustained attention even when presented with distracters. In examining
iPad use effects on attention, this study hypothesizes that due to the many exogenous interactive
stimuli and constantly changing visual stimulation in iPad apps and games, children may not
learn to inhibit visual distracters, which may slow their development of visual spatial attention
span in circumstances without rapid exogenous stimulation.
Television Viewing and Attention Effects
The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) discourages the use of media for children
two years old and younger (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2011). However, despite this clear
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
8!
recommendation, infant and toddler media use remains prevalent (Radesky, Silverstein,
Zuckerman, & Christakis, 2014). Many parents steadfastly believe that there are beneficial
effects of early exposure to television and computers. A 2006 survey of 1,000 US families with
children between 2 and 24 months old revealed that the leading justification parents gave for
allowing infant video/DVD viewing is that such media use “teaches him/her something or is
good for his/her brain” (Zack, Barr, Gerhardstein, Dickerson, & Meltzoff, 2009). Although
current research has found educational television and videos to be ineffective for children 30
months and younger, this belief remains prevalent (Anderson and Hanson, 2013). Given the
popularity of mobile media and parental beliefs in the benefits of early childhood media use,
there is an urgent need for research on how mobile media use influences attention span
development in young children.
A review of the current body of research on TV screen time effects on children reveals
that a majority of studies found a negative association between amount of screen time and
sustained attention span (Anderson & Hanson, 2013; Radesky, Silverstein, Zuckerman, &
Christakis, 2014; Landhuis, Poulton, Welch, & Hancox, 2007; Christakis, Zimmerman,
DiGiuseppe, & McCarty, 2004; Zimmerman & Christakis, 2005; Hornik, 1981; Singer, 1980;
Swing, Gentile, Anderson, & Walsh, 2010; Kumari & Ahuja, 2010). As an example, a general
population longitudinal study tracking children’s television viewing at ages 5, 7, 9, and 11 years
found a positive association between hours of TV viewed in childhood and severity of attention
problems in adolescence (Landhuis, Poulton, Welch, & Hancox, 2007). This association is
significant even after controlling for gender, attention problems in early childhood, cognitive
ability at 5 years of age, and childhood socioeconomic status. The results of this study indicate
that childhood television viewing may contribute to the development of long-lasting attention
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
9!
problems into adolescence. This association found between amount of TV use and subsequent
attention problems leads to the hypothesis that amount of mobile media iPad use is positively
associated with level of sustained attention problems. However, researchers’ inability to assess
whether preschool viewing had an impact on attention, due to lack of data retrieved from before
age 5, poses a limitation to this study. Researchers are also unable to pinpoint which aspects of
television viewing may have contributed to the attention problems, and whether or not type of
programming, such as entertaining versus educational, had different effects on attention.
Because screen time for many children begins before the age of 5, researching preschool
viewing impacts on attention is important. Zimmerman and Christakis (2005) examined the
effects of such early toddler and preschool television viewing. They researched the effects of TV
and video exposure at ages 2, 3, and 5, on children’s cognitive outcomes at ages 6 and 7. Using
data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 Children and Young Adults
(NLSYChild), researchers drew a sample of all children who were approximately 6 years old.
They recorded hours of television watched per day prior to 6 years old for the three following
predictor variables: television viewing before 3 years of age, viewing from ages 3 to 5, and
viewing at age 6. Researchers then assessed cognitive outcome measurements using the Peabody
Individual Achievement Test and the Memory for Digit Span assessment from the Wechsler
Intelligence Scales for Children. Their results indicate that television viewing before 3 years of
age positively correlates with adverse cognitive outcomes on reading skills and comprehension,
focus and attention, and memory at ages 6 and 7. These associations remained significant even
after controlling for parental investment in cognitive outcomes, innate cognitive ability from
parents, race, ethnicity, language spoken, income level, parent’s education, number of people in
the household, gender, participation in Head Start, and rural versus urban residence.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
10!
Radesky, Silverstein, Zuckerman, and Christakis’ (2014) study corroborates this research.
They found that media exposure is associated with later problems in language development,
cognition, attention, executive functioning, and school achievement. They found these effects to
be more pronounced with higher levels of media exposure and lower educational content of
programs viewed. The researchers were also able to propose causes for these detrimental effects,
positing that TV replaced enriching activities with adults and peers, reduced play and language-
based interactions with parents, and resulted in less active and creative child play. If early infant
and toddler exposure to TV has been shown to negatively affect cognitive outcomes, such
research supports the hypothesis that early exposure to iPad mobile media, a more pervasive
form of screen time, is also likely to have detrimental cognitive outcomes.
The research on the effects of toddler and preschool television viewing succeeds in
identifying negative cognitive outcomes associated with greater amounts of media exposure.
However, neither study examined what characteristics of TV specifically influenced cognitive
effects, nor focused exclusively on attention outcomes. Christakis, Zimmerman, DiGiuseppe, and
McCarty (2004) addressed this gap in the literature and theorized that the rapid sequencing of
changes during television and video programming may influence neurological development.
They hypothesized that early exposure to television during the critical periods of synaptic
development, ages 1 and 3, would be associated with subsequent attention problems at age 7.
The results of their study indicate that early television viewing is associated with subsequent
attention problems. During the first few years of childhood, the brain has considerable plasticity
and therefore may be overstimulated by rapid image and scene changes commonly found in TV.
Such overstimulation can adversely affect brain development by affecting the number and
density of neuronal synapses. Even though TV may be overstimulating, children find it
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
11!
extremely engaging with its often rapidly changing images, scenery, and events and exercise
exogenous attention to focus on the screen. Children may find reality to be less eventful or
boring in comparison to life portrayed on TV with its attention-grabbing stimuli and fast-paced
editing (Hornik, 1981; Singer,1980). Therefore, for children, watching large amounts of TV may
lead to lower tolerance of slower-paced, more mundane tasks or tasks that require more
endogenous attention. Children would thus develop shorter sustained attention spans when not
engaged with screen time. A limitation to this study, however, is that content of television
viewing is not addressed. It is possible that educational versus entertaining content may influence
attention span. This proposed study will examine content of iPad use in terms of level of
endogenous and exogenous stimuli in each app used by children in order to account for possible
moderating effects.
Despite the large body of work that has found associations between screen time exposure
and decreasing attention span, the association remains inconclusive because some studies have
shown no association between the two variables. For example, Foster and Watking (2010) found
no meaningful relation between television viewing and attention problems in children. Using
data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (N = 1,159), this study reexamined the link
between television viewing at ages 1 and 3 and attention problems at age 7 that Christakis,
Zimmerman, DiGiuseppe, and McCarty (2004) found. These researchers found that the
association between early television viewing at ages 1 and 3 and risk of attention problems
measured at age 7 is a nonlinear relationship such that the association is significant only for the
10% of the sample who watched 7 or more hours of TV per day. In addition, researchers found
that when controlling for measures of maternal academic achievement and family income in the
child’s early life, the associations between television viewing and attention problems are no
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
12!
longer significant, thus contradicting the Zimmerman and Christakis’ (2005) results that
suggested a significant effect even after controlling for these variables. Therefore, overall, the
effects of media use and screen time on attention development needs to be further researched in
order to draw a more reliable conclusion. Researching mobile media iPad effects is needed
because it is the new most prevalent form of media use for children in the US.
Video Games and Attention Effects
While research on TV can inform future research on mobile media use, a large difference
exists between TV use and mobile media iPad use. TV viewing is a passive activity whereas
mobile media iPad use entails interactive engagement. An existing form of interactive media that
has been around longer than mobile media is video games. While video game effects on
development have not been as thoroughly researched as TV viewing, the following studies are
important for examining the association between a form of interactive screen time use and
children’s sustained attention span. Swing, Gentle, Anderson, and Walsh (2010) used a sample
of 1,323 middle childhood participants, ages 7 through 11, and 210 late adolescent/early adult
participants to study the association between video games and attention problems across age.
For the middle childhood participants, researchers collected parent-and-child reports regarding
amount of video game exposure, and assessed attention problems using teacher-reports on
behavior. As for the early adult/late adolescent participants, they self-reported amount of video
game use and completed a battery of tests to assess their mental health status. Researchers found
that the amount of time spent playing video games is positively associated with levels of
attention problems for both groups. While the association between video game use and attention
problems is similar in strength to the association with passive television viewing for both middle
childhood and late adolescence and early-adulthood groups, in middle childhood the video game
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
13!
association is more consistent than the television association in predicting attention problems.
Thus, this research suggests that interactive forms of media are likely to have more prominent
and predictable effects on attention span. In addition, due to the use of the two different age
groups, the study also suggests that attention effects are observed across ages from middle
childhood to early adulthood, and that such consequences may be long lasting or cumulative.
Kumari and Ahuja’s (2010) findings on video game use effects on cognitive functions,
including attention span, supports these results. In a study of children from ages 9 to 12, they
found that heavy video game use is also associated with poorer performances on tests of
attention span, creative imagination, and visual memory in comparison to light video game users.
They proposed that these negative effects are found because children’s video games, due to their
constantly changing and interactive stimuli, demand that their viewers make constant attention
shifts. This type of interaction with media does not require children to exercise sustained,
prolonged attention to real-life events. These studies on video game use, however, similar to the
television viewing studies, did not take into account the content of video game programming
being used. Therefore, educational video games will possibly have different cognitive effects on
children than entertaining video games.
Current iPad Research
While research on TV and video game screen time can inform and guide research on new
mobile media, it is faulty to assume that TV, video games, and mobile media have the same
degree of effects. Mobile media differs from other forms of media in several ways that may
influence its effects on attention span. Unlike television, the iPad has multiple modalities and
interactive capabilities (Radesky, Schumacher, & Zuckerman, 2015). In comparison to both TV
and video games, iPads are also more pervasive and thus are ubiquitous in childrens lives. They
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
14!
are more easily accessible to children because they are portable and lightweight, and therefore
can be brought to and used in a variety of settings and locations (Kucirkova, 2014). In addition,
due to their touch screen function, iPads eliminate the need for external devices, such as a mouse
and keyboard, that require certain levels of dexterity. IPads are also designed to accommodate a
number of apps, many of which are created to appeal to children through child-friendly intuitive
designs.
A review of current interactive mobile media research shows that little research has been
done to study mobile media longitudinal effects on visual spatial attention span. The existing
body of iPad research focuses instead on social emotional development effects and media use
specifically as an educational tool. Radesky, Schumacher, and Zuckerman (2015) studied how
parents are using mobile media as a behavioral regulation tool in order to keep them pacified and
engaged during outings. These researchers explained, however, that although mobile media
devices may be helpful in the short term for occupying children, overuse of such devices to
pacify children could be harmful to later social-emotional outcomes. Children may be less likely
to develop the internal mechanism of self-regulation if they are not given the opportunity to calm
themselves down without the aide of a media device. Mobile media may have a similar
association with attention span, such that if the child is exposed to mobile media as a behavior
regulation tool in many different settings, the child will be less likely to develop sustained
attention or focus on at-hand tasks and situations. Mobile media would serve as a constant
distracter by offering constantly shifting stimuli that would prevent children from having to
maintain sustained attention to any stimuli for too long.
In terms of research regarding iPad functioning specifically as an educational tool,
studies have shown contrasting results for the effectiveness of interactive media on learning and
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
15!
reading comprehension. In a study on mobile media book format effects on reading
comprehension, children are found to comprehend significantly less when reading electronic
books than when reading traditional book. Electronic book reading is associated with an increase
in parent distraction talk about book format and technology use (Krcmar & Cingel, 2014).
During electronic book reading, there is less parent talk focused on the plot and story elements.
However, Kucirkova (2014) found a contrasting effect, such that iPad electronic books and
interactive learn-to-read apps are associated with an increase in early literacy skills by providing
additional practice with word and letter recognition and phonics. However, researchers found
that this effectiveness of iPads is contingent on the context of use, such that teachers’ ability to
effectively link the iPad tasks to the curriculum is directly related to the learning potential of the
iPad. Therefore, while iPad use may have the potential to produce positive cognitive effects, it is
only when there is direct instruction and interaction with an adult or teacher that these effects are
found. Everyday use of an iPad is likely to show no educational benefits, even with the use of
educational apps, without the social interaction and instructive component. Using the iPad by
oneself has not been shown to promote reading comprehension, most likely because the media
form and the electronic book enhancements deter the child’s attention to the story (Roseberry,
Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2014). Building from this research of a decreased focus on story plot
when reading stories on mobile media, other activities performed on the iPad, especially if not
educationally focused, may associate with stimulating less sustained, focused attention in
children.
Research on the educational effectiveness of the iPad has found that the touch screen is
an innovative and unique feature. A touch screen allows young children easy access to mobile
media use, as advanced motor skills are not required to operate an iPad that functions mainly
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
16!
through finger-swipes and taps. Research conducted on whether or not what children learn from
a touch screen can be replicated in real life has found that learning does not transfer from 2D
objects and symbols to 3D counterparts for young children as easily as it does from within-
dimension (2D/2D or 3D/3D) conditions (Zack, Barr, Gerhardstein, Dickerson, & Meltzoff,
2009). Using a sample of seventy-two fifteen to sixteen-month-old infants, researchers had
infants either participate in a cross-dimensions condition, during which infants would observe an
action on a 2D display and then be prompted to imitate the action on a 3D object and vice versa,
or they would participate in a within-dimension condition, during which infants would imitate an
action observed on a 2D screen on a similar 2D setup, or similarly with a 3D object. They found
that the cross-dimension groups performed significantly worse than the within-dimension
groups, suggesting a video deficit in transferring learning from 2D to 3D and vice versa.
Although attention effects are not examined in this study, similar to learning, attention may not
transfer from 2D interactions to 3D interactions. If attention span thus does not translate from
media use to reality, persistent iPad use may result in children’s attention span being contingent
on immediate reward and engaging stimuli, both of which are characteristics not commonly
found in other daily activities such as school work or social play.
IPad Use and Attention
While the previous studies have addressed children’s attention while using a mobile
media device, they have not addressed what elements of the iPad have made them so appealing
to young children, and thus more frequently used. Christakis (2014) explored why iPad apps and
interaction in general can capture the attention of young children and lead to excessive mobile
media use. He theorized that tablet and iPad games are similar to interactive toys with which
babies learn cause and effect by receiving the reward of making something happen by pressing a
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
17!
button. He describes it as the rewarding feeling of "I did it!" Christakis (2014) claims that it is
both edifying and potentially addictive for a child when they derive pleasure from touching a
screen and making something happen, and thus they can remain attentive to the iPad for long
periods of time. However, Christakis (2014) fails to address whether sustained attention fostered
during iPad use translates to different activities and tasks apart from mobile media that often do
not offer an immediate reward.
Current research on mobile media and iPads overall has shown that context of interactive
mobile media is significant in determining media effects on children, and that content of media
use has the potential to influence such effects. However, in the majority of studies that have
researched the effects of media use on attention, data on content of programs viewed, such as
educational versus entertaining, are not recorded or factored into data analysis. Therefore, it is
important that such variables be assessed in this study’s research on the effects of mobile media
use in early childhood. In addition, while most of the studies reviewed did examine iPad use in
early childhood, there is a gap in the literature of long-term cognitive effects of using these
devices extensively during early years of life. According to Radesky, Schumacher, and
Zuckerman (2015), researching effects of mobile media use for infants, toddlers, and
preschoolers is especially vital because such effects are potentially more prominent for these
younger groups going through major developmental periods in their lives. This study will
address the literature gap of early childhood iPad use effects on cognitive development,
specifically visual spatial attention span.
Proposed Study
In this cross-sectional correlational study, researchers will investigate how mobile media
use in early childhood affects development of visual spatial attention span in young children.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
18!
Specifically they will examine how the interactivity, accessibility, and portability of iPads
influence the amount of mobile media use, and how that amount in turn affects attention span. In
addition, researchers will collect data on the content of media use by looking at the levels of
exogenous stimuli present in most frequently used apps and viewed TV programs. They will
recruit a sample of children ages 3 to 6 who began media use at age 3 or younger, and categorize
the sample into three groups within their age range: TV viewers only, interactive iPad users, and
passive iPad users. Children from all groups will arrive at the lab where they will take an
attention test while parents fill out questionnaires regarding child media use. Parents will report
information on average weekly amount of iPad or TV use, most frequently used iPad apps or
viewed TV programs, and context and location of iPad use. Parents will also complete a survey
of family background and history to control for confounding variables. Researchers will assess
the length of visual spatial attention span for each child upon completion of the attention test,
and then perform data analysis to detect associations between parent-reported media use
variables and length of sustained attention. In order to see potential long-term effects of iPad use
on attention, researchers will compare data from the different ages to assess possible trends.
IPads are designed to be portable, lightweight, easy to use, and easily accessible in nearly
every location. These new devices have thus become prevalent in children’s lives as parents find
them to be an easy method of occupying their child whether at home or out of the house.
Researchers hypothesize that increased use of the iPad in various contexts outside of the home,
coupled with its enticing interactive features and engaging stimuli, will correlate with increased
amounts of iPad use. Increased amounts of iPad use in early childhood may be associated with
higher levels of sustained attention problems. In addition, content of apps used in terms of level
of exogenous factors is also likely to influence attention. Researchers predict that interactive iPad
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
19!
use is more likely to have more exogenous stimuli, which could lead to higher levels of attention
problems due to constant contact with exogenous stimuli, which in turn hinder the development
of endogenous attention needed for sustained attention in tasks apart from mobile media. In
contrast, passive iPad use is likely to correlate with lower levels of exogenous stimuli.
Researchers hypothesize that an increased amount of iPad use coupled with interactive use high
in exogenous stimuli will have a greater negative effect on sustained attention.
The variables of amount, context, and type of use will differentiate iPad use from TV in
terms of amount of hours spent with each form of media, where and how each media form is
used, and interactive or passive use of the media. Researchers predict that these factors will
correlate with the strength of effect each media form has on attention span. They also
hypothesize that because of these variables, even passive use of iPads could have greater effects
on attention than passive viewing of TV due to more frequent use in various contexts.
Researchers will use the data collected on the content of iPad app use in terms of level of
exogenous stimuli in order to detect differences in attention span between passive iPad use and
interactive iPad use. Finally, researchers predict that mobile media iPad use effects on length of
visual spatial attention span will be found at all ages throughout early childhood.
Proposed Method
Participants
Researchers performed a power analysis in order to determine the number of child
participants needed for this study. First, they determined a medium effect size from previous
research on television viewing effects on attention conducted by Landhuis, Poulton, Welch, &
Hancox (2007). Using Cohen’s (1992) power table, for a power of .8 with a medium effect size
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
20!
and an alpha of .05, researchers established that this study will need 160 participants. These 160
participants will include children between the ages 3 and 6 living in two-parent households who
began media use at age 3 or younger. There will be approximately 40 children in each age range:
3 to 3 years and 11 months, 4 to 4 years and 11 months, 5 to 5 years and 11 months, and 6 to 6
years and 11 months. Participants will have access to iPads or/and television within the home.
Children with ADHD/ADD, autism, or other developmental or neurological delay diagnoses will
be excluded from the study. All participants will also have normal visual acuity, or corrected to
normal.
Participants will be recruited so that there are three groups of media users for each age
range: passive iPad users, interactive iPad users, and TV use only users. Passive iPad users are
defined as those who use the iPad as a program-viewing device without manipulation or
utilization of the touchscreen during program duration. For example, children who watch
YouTube videos or movies on the iPad are passive users. Interactive users are defined as those
who utilize the touchscreen function to physically manipulate what is on the screen during the
duration of app use on the iPad. For example, children who play games such as Angry Birds on
the iPad where swiping motions on the touchscreen is necessary will be determined as interactive
users.
Pew Research Center found that demographic groups that are most likely to own tablets
include those in households earning at least $75,000 per year, adults with at least a college
degree, those living in suburban areas, and parents with minor children living at home (Tablet
Ownership 2013, 2013). Due to these statistics, researchers predict that the child participant base
will be representative of these demographic groups. In addition, Pew Research Center found that
there are no statistically significant differences in tablet ownership between members of different
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
21!
racial or ethnic groups. To control for parent education level, only children whose parents have
an undergraduate degree and no higher degree will be recruited. Of the child participants,
approximately half will be female and half will be male. Participants will be recruited from a
college town and therefore the average annual household income may be slightly higher than that
of the general population. Participants will be recruited via flyers in pediatric offices, local
preschools, daycare centers, elementary schools, churches, recreation centers, youth centers,
local newspaper ads, and parks within a seven-mile radius of the university. In terms of
compensation, each child will receive a toy upon exiting the lab. In addition, parents will be
compensated $30 along with a booklet of coupons to local children boutiques and stores.
Materials
This study will include a visual spatial attention span assessment tool along with
measurements for average amount of media use, age of child, context of media use, and content
of media use.
Visual Spatial Attention Span. Children’s visual spatial attention span will be assessed
using the rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) subtest from the Early Childhood Attention
Battery (ECAB; Breckenridge, Braddick, & Atkinson, 2013). This subtest is a computer task that
will be administered via a Dell laptop computer with a screen size of 28.7 centimeters by 21.7
centimeters. The laptop will be placed at a 35 centimeter viewing distance. Brightness, contrast,
and luminance of screen for each picture will be kept constant for each trial. In this RSVP
subtest, a continuous stream of picture stimuli will be presented on the computer screen. Each
stimulus has a duration of 200 ms with an inter-stimulus interval of 1800 ms. The child will be
asked to verbally report when any of the following five target animals appear: cat, dog, pig,
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
22!
horse, and fish. The animal targets will be randomly interspersed among non-animal, familiar
non-target items with monosyllabic names including car, bike, tree, hat, shoe, shirt, ball, doll,
tooth, pie, juice, and slide. Figure 2 shows the stimuli that will be used in this computer task.
Children will be shown the target animal images and told the names of the animals in order to
ensure that they know what they are looking for. Children will take initial practice tests until they
feel comfortable with fulfilling the requirements of this task.
Figure!2.!Target!and!nonGtarget!stimuli!!
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
23!
One hundred twenty non-targets and 30 targets will be presented over a 5-minute session.
Two research assistants will record the child’s verbal reports. Two researchers will keep record
in order to increase the reliability of data collection. If a child misses four consecutive targets,
they will receive a prompt to pay attention. The number of correct responses, commission errors,
and prompts will be recorded. Commission errors are determined as responses to non-targets.
Overall score on this assessment will be calculated by number of correct responses minus
number of commission errors and prompts. The higher the overall score, the higher the level of
visual sustained attention (see Appendix A for the RSVP subtest model). Breckenridge,
Braddick, and Atkinson (2013) found the ECAB to have good reliability ( r = 0.75) and good
convergent validity (r = 0.77).
Amount of Media Use. Caregivers will report how many hours their child uses an iPad
on a typical weekday and a typical weekend day. A composite score will be calculated to find the
average daily amount of iPad use. This average daily amount of iPad use will be calculated by
multiplying the weekday response by five, the weekend day response by two, adding the two
results, and then dividing by seven. Parents who report that their child uses the iPad for more
than 16 hours per day will be dropped from analysis, because such responses have been found to
be non-credible (Christakis, Zimmerman, DiGiuseppe, & McCarty, 2004; Zimmerman &
Christakis, 2005). The same question formatting will be used to determine TV use by replacing
the word “iPad” with “TV.
Pervasiveness of Media Use. Pervasiveness of iPad use and TV use will relate to the
settings in which children have access to media and how often children use media in these
settings. Caregivers will record how often the child uses the iPad or views TV outside the home,
in the car, at a restaurant, during social gatherings, and while waiting for services either in a line
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
24!
or in a waiting room. Responses will be recorded using a Likert scale from 1-4 (1- never, 4–
always; See Appendix B for exact measure of context of media use). Composite scores will be
calculated by taking the average of the scores. Higher scores are indicative of more pervasive use
of media in daily life. In addition a factor analysis will be preformed to make sure all of the items
for this measure are measuring the same thing.
Content of Media. Content of media use will refer to the level of exogenous stimuli in
the TV programs children watch and in the programs or apps children use on the iPad.
Caregivers of the TV-use-only group will report what three TV programs their child most
frequently watches. Caregivers of iPad use groups will report what three programs and apps their
child most frequently uses. Research assistants who have been trained in research ethics and
have shown high inter-rater reliability in identifying exogenous stimuli will access the programs
and apps and determine their level of exogenous stimuli using a Likert scale from one to four (1-
no exogenous stimuli, 4- all exogenous stimuli). Composite scores for the content of use will be
calculated by taking the average of the three scores.
Age. Caregivers will report the current age of the child to the nearest month, for example,
3 years and 9 months old. Caregivers will choose year and month options from a drop down
menu. The year drop down menu will include the numbers 3, 4, 5, and 6. The month drop down
menu will include numbers 1 through 11.
Demographic Information. Caregivers will complete a demographic background
survey, the responses of which will be used to control for the following covariates: gender, race
and ethnicity, socioeconomic status, gestational age of child at birth, birth weight, maternal use
of alcohol or tobacco during pregnancy, language spoken in the home, amount of media use
within the last week, and starting age of media use (see Appendix C for the complete survey).
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
25!
These covariates are controlled for in previous research on media use and attention effects
(Radesky, Silverstein, Zuckerman, & Christakis, 2014; Zimmerman & Christakis, 2005), and
therefore are also predicted to be influential in this study on iPad use and attention . Gender of
the child will be a dichotomous variable of male or female. For race and ethnicity, the following
choices will be available: White (non-Latino), Latino, Asian/Asian American, Pacific Islander,
African American/Black, Native American/Alaskan Native, or Mixed. Socioeconomic status will
be determined by reported annual household income. Caregivers will record the gestational age
of their child at birth in months, birth weight in pounds to the nearest ounce, whether the mother
used alcohol or tobacco during pregnancy (any or none), and language spoken in the home
(English vs. non-English). For determining amount of media use in the last week, parents will
report how many hours their child used the iPad or viewed TV within the past week, As for
calculating starting age of use, caregivers will record at what age, to the nearest year, did their
child start using the iPad or view TV.
Procedure
Upon arriving at the laboratory and after parents provide informed consent and child
participants provide assent, the child will enter a room with a one-way mirror where he or she
will engage in a five-minute socialization period with a research assistant. After the socialization
period the research assistant will explain the ECAB visual sustained attention computer task
instructions.
While the child is completing the ECAB subtest, caregivers will stay behind the one-way
mirror. First, they will report to research assistants the three most frequently used iPad apps or
viewed TV shows by their child. Then, caregivers will enter the following predictor variable
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
26!
information into a computer: current age of child, average weekday and weekend day media use,
and context of use. Following that data collection, parents will complete the demographics
survey. Children who complete the attention assessment before parents have completed their
surveys will be given a toy to play with in the lab room until parents are finished.
At the end of the session, participants will be fully debriefed and children and parents
will be compensated.
Ethics
Due to the increasing prevalence of young children’s iPad and mobile media use, it is
imperative to study the effects of these new media devices on children’s attention span. Very
little research has been done to date on the long-term cognitive effects of mobile media use in
early childhood. This proposed research will attempt to address these gaps in the literature. This
study will use a sample of one hundred and sixty children from ages three to six. Although these
children are a protected population, it is necessary to have a sample of children within this age
range in order to understand how mobile media use influences attention span in early childhood,
and how such effects may persist throughout those years. An increase in amount of iPad mobile
media use has been found in early childhood (Common Sense Media, 2013), and therefore it is
crucial to study this target population. Safeguards will be put in place to protect this population,
such as enforcing that all participation be voluntary. Participants will be the children of the
caregivers who respond to flyers calling for participants for the study. Caregivers will be
required to give informed consent and children will be required to give assent before any testing
takes place. Therefore, both the adult and child will have to agree that they want to proceed with
the study.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
27!
After the child and adult give their assent and consent, they will partake in the minimal
risk study. All data collection will be anonymous. Parents and children will be assigned matching
numbers upon arrival at the lab, so that parent survey data and child test data can be paired
during data analysis. No names or identifying information will be collected other than gender
and ethnicity of the participants. Because of the expected diversity in the sample group, gender
and ethnicity will not allow for participant identification. After receiving their number
assignments, the children will perform an attention test. The test will take place on a computer.
The child merely has to respond to the changing picture prompts on the screen and verbally
exclaim when he or she sees an animal picture. The distractor prompts will consist of pictures of
daily household objects including a car, bike, tree, hat, shoe, shirt, ball, doll, tooth, pie, juice, and
slide. The target prompt will include five animals: cat, dog, pig, horse, and fish. These prompts
are low risk because they are objects and animals a child is familiar with and may encounter on a
daily basis.
While the child is taking the test, parents will remain outside the room behind a one-way
mirror, completing a survey on media use and family background and history that asks for no
sensitive information. Therefore, these surveys are also minimal risk. If the child is in distress
about being away from his or her parent, the parent will be allowed to stand at the back of the
room. Parent placement will be noted and accounted for in data analysis for possible
confounding effects, if such a situation were to occur. This protocol adds additional protection
for the children by working to minimize and prevent any distress they may have. Furthermore,
no deception will be involved in this study. The procedure will consist only of an attention test
completed by the child, and the measurement and demographics surveys by the parent. Upon
completing the study tasks at the end of the lab session, participants will be fully debriefed and
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
28!
compensated. There are expected to be no long term-consequences on the child or parent upon
completion of their participation.
Because of the dearth in research on this topic, there are few empirically based
recommended guidelines for caregivers on how to regulate mobile media use for young children.
The benefits of conducting the proposed research include using the results to help create
guidelines for parents, teachers, and pediatricians on the best and most appropriate way to
monitor mobile media use for children in early childhood. This study will give crucial insight
into how mobile media use affects visual spatial attention span in early childhood. Because
young children’s capacity to explore and learn about their visual world is dependent on their
ability to hold their attention, it is important for caregivers to understand the potential
consequences and risks of mobile media use on children’s visual spatial attention span
development (Kannass, Oakes, & Shaddy, 2006). Therefore, the benefits of this study outweigh
the potential risks.
Analytics
Before testing the hypotheses, researchers will perform descriptive analyses in order to
check for normality of data and heteroscedasticity of errors. If the data is not normally
distributed, researchers will appropriately transform the data. If the errors are heteroscedastic,
researchers will perform the appropriate nonparametric tests. For the purposes of this proposed
study, researchers will assume normality of data and homoscedasticity of errors. Therefore,
researchers will present the results from the appropriate parametric tests. In addition, researchers
will control for the following covariates in each test: gender, race and ethnicity, socioeconomic
status, gestational age of child at birth, birth weight, maternal use of alcohol or tobacco during
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
29!
pregnancy, language spoken in the home, amount of media use within the last week, and starting
age of media use.
Researchers will perform a multiple regression with level of pervasiveness, a continuous
predictor, participant group, a contrast-coded predictor, and amount of media use as the
dependent variable. As both predictors are continuous, a multiple regression is appropriate.
Researchers will use a multiple regression to test the hypothesis that, while controlling for level
of pervasiveness, iPad use groups correlate with longer amounts of media use in comparison to
TV use groups. This analysis will also test whether an interaction between participant group and
level of pervasiveness exists due to higher levels of pervasiveness correlating with longer
amounts of media use.
Researchers will conduct another multiple regression with amount of media use, level of
pervasiveness of use, content of use, and age, all continuous predictors, and with participant
group, a contrast-coded predictor. The continuous dependent variable will be visual spatial
attention span. This analysis will test the hypothesis that, while controlling for age, the content of
media use correlates with length of visual spatial attention span. This analysis will also test the
hypothesis that participant group correlates with length of visual spatial attention span, even
while controlling for amount of media use. In addition, because longer amounts of media use are
expected to correlate with shorter visual spatial attention spans, this analysis will test whether an
interaction exists between the amount of media use and participant group. Furthermore, this
analysis will test the hypothesis that the interactive iPad user group, in combination with an
interaction of high amounts of media use, high levels of pervasiveness, and high levels of
exogenous stimuli will have the strongest correlation with shorter visual spatial attention spans
relative to all other groups.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
30!
Predicted Results
First, researchers will perform a typical log transformation on the RSVP data and conduct
an outlier analysis. Any outliers will be removed from data analysis. Next, researchers will
calculate composite scores for the following predictor variables: pervasiveness of use, content of
use, and amount of use. For each subsequent analysis, consistent with previous literature on
media use and effects on attention, researchers will control for the following covariates: gender,
race and ethnicity, socioeconomic status, gestational age of child at birth, birth weight, maternal
use of alcohol or tobacco during pregnancy, language spoken in the home, amount of media use
within the last week, and starting age of media use (Zimmerman & Christakis, 2005; Radesky,
Silverstein, Zuckerman, & Christakis, 2014).
In order to first establish that iPad use correlates with longer amounts of media use in
comparison to TV, researchers will perform a multiple regression using the following predictor
variables: participant group and level of pervasiveness. The predictor variable participant group
consists of the following three groups: interactive iPad users, passive iPad users, and TV
viewers. The dependent variable in this analysis will be the amount of media use. Researchers
predict that there will be a significant main effect such that when controlling for level of
pervasiveness, participant group will have an effect on amount of media consumption. Both
groups of iPad users are expected to demonstrate longer amounts of media use than TV use. This
prediction is based on what is known about how iPads are designed to increase their appeal to
children. IPad’s touchscreen interactive interface eliminates the need for more advanced
dexterity to manipulate external devices when using a media device, thus allowing for easier
child use and access (Kucirkova, 2014). Also, iPads are exclusively designed to accommodate
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
31!
numerous apps, many of which are created to appeal directly to children. These features would
increase the appeal of iPad use and encourage longer use in comparison to TV viewing.
Researchers also expect to find an interaction between the variables such that the level of
pervasiveness moderates the relationship between participant group and amount of media used.
For example, interactive iPad users with higher levels of pervasive media use will have used
media for longer amounts of time, whereas interactive iPad users with lower levels of pervasive
media use will have used media for shorter amounts of time. This prediction that iPad user
groups spend more time with media is based on research that has found that iPads’ portability
and easy-to-use features increase the likelihood for higher consumption levels among children
(Radesky, Schumacher, & Zuckerman, 2015). In addition, iPad user groups are more likely to
have more time with media because of parents’ tendency to use iPads as a behavioral regulation
tool (Radesky, Schumacher, & Zuckerman, 2015).
In order to test for the predictor variable effects on visual spatial attention span,
researchers will perform a multiple regression with the following predictor variables: participant
group, amount of media use, level of pervasiveness of use, content of use, and age. The
dependent variable will be visual spatial attention span. Researchers first expect to find a
negative correlation between the amount of media use and length of visual spatial attention span,
such that longer amount of media use will be associated with shorter visual spatial attention span.
Previous research on TV use and attention has shown a similar association, such that increased
amounts of TV viewing is associated with higher rates of attention problems (Landhuis, Poulton,
Welch, & Hancox, 2007; Christakis, Zimmerman, DiGiuseppe, & McCarty, 2004).
In addition, researchers hypothesize that the multiple regression will reveal a main effect
between age of the child and content of media use. They expect that controlling for age of the
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
32!
child, content of use will correlate with visual spatial attention span such that higher levels of
exogenous stimuli will correlate with shorter visual spatial attention spans. This prediction is
based on attention development theory research that has found that stimuli with high levels of
exogenous stimupi—novel movements, contrasts, surprises, complexity, and incongruity--can
hamper children’s abilities to learn to inhibit visual distracters (Ruff and Capozzoli, 2003).
Kannass, Oakes, and Shaddy (2006) also found that children who are effective at inhibiting
responses to distracters are able to maintain longer attention span. Researchers also hypothesize
that there will be no interaction between age and content of use such that the relationship
between high levels of exogenous stimuli and shorter visual spatial attention span will remain
constant for all ages. There will be no decrease in the effects of exposure to exogenous stimuli
with age. This prediction is grounded in attention development theory that states that around 3
years of age, typical development should show children exhibiting more focused, sustained
attention even when presented with distracters (Kannass, Oakes, and Shaddy, 2006).!Children at
3 who are exposed to high levels of exogenous stimuli through media use are less likely to be
able to inhibit distracters and thus would not develop sustained attention. If children continue to
engage with exogenous stimuli as they age, they will not develop the skills to inhibit visual
distracters and thus will continue to have short visual spatial attention spans. Researchers also
predict that the interactive iPad user group will correlate with the highest levels of exogenous
stimuli relative to the other two groups. This prediction is based on research on video games that
has shown that interactive media incorporate perceptually salient and constantly changing stimuli
that demands viewers to make constant attention shifts (Kumari and Ahuja’s 2010). This type of
interaction with media in turn prevents children from exercising sustained, prolonged attention to
real-life events.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
33!
Noting the association between participant group and amount of media use, researchers
hypothesize that there will be a main effect between the two variables and how they influence
visual spatial attention span. Controlling for amount of media consumption, researchers expect
participant group to correlate with the length of visual spatial attention span such that interactive
iPad user groups will exhibit the shorter visual spatial attention spans relative to passive iPad
users and TV users. Researchers’s prediction is based on knowledge of the prior established
association between interactive iPad use and exposure to higher levels of exogenous stimuli that
have been shown to lead to shorter visual spatial attention spans (Kumari and Ahuja’s 2010).
Researchers also expect an interaction between amount of use and participant group that
influences visual spatial attention span. Longer amounts of use will correlate with shorter
attention span, and participant group will moderate this relationship. Researchers predict that
longer amounts of use combined with either of the iPad user groups will be associated with
shorter visual spatial attention spans relative to longer amounts of use combined with TV users.
They also hypothesize that of the three participant groups, the interaction between longer
amounts of use and interactive iPad group users will have the strongest association to shorter
visual spatial attention spans. This prediction is based on research that has found a correlation
between iPad use and high levels of pervasiveness in children’s lives, as well as research that has
significant correlations between longer amounts of interactive media use and shorter visual
spatial attention spans (Radesky, Schumacher, & Zuckerman, 2015; Kumari & Ahuja, 2010;
Zimmerman & Christakis, 2005).
Overall, researchers predict that the multiple regression will reveal that the interactive
iPad user group, in combination with an interaction of high amounts of media use, high levels of
pervasiveness, and high levels of exogenous stimuli, will have the strongest correlation with
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
34!
shorter visual spatial attention spans relative to all other groups. Figure 3 shows the predicted
lengths of attention span for each participant group.
Figure 3. !Media!Use!Effects!on!Visual!Spatial!Attention!Span
!
Overall, researchers predict that the multiple regression will reveal that the interactive
iPad user group, in combination with an interaction of high amounts of media use, high levels of
pervasiveness, and high levels of exogenous stimuli, will have the strongest correlation with
shorter visual spatial attention spans relative to all other groups. Figure 3 shows the predicted
lengths of attention span for each participant group.
Discussion
The ability to maintain a sustained attention span is crucial to a young child’s capacity to
learn about the visual world around them (Kannass, Oakes, & Shaddy, 2006). Young children
rely on attention to gather and manage the information they need to navigate and learn about
Interactive!iPad!Users!
TV!Use!Only!
Length!of!Visual!Spatial!Attention!Span!
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
35!
their environment. The development of such sustained attention begins in early childhood and
can be fostered or hindered by environment and upbringing. Childhood media use is one factor
that affects children’s attention span. As the entertainment industry becomes more
technologically advanced, media is becoming more portable and easily accessed. With the rise of
mobile media since 2010, children are increasingly gaining more access to media at increasingly
younger ages (Common Sense Media, 2013). Despite the rising prevalence of mobile media in
children’s lives, few guidelines exist to inform parents and educators how to monitor or use such
technology with young children. Researchers expect that this study will give insight to caregivers
on how mobile media use, specifically iPad use, in early childhood affects visual spatial attention
span. !
Researchers predict that in comparison to TV viewing, early exposure to iPad use will be
associated with shorter visual spatial attention spans. This effect will be constant across ages 3
through 6, such that visual spatial attention span deficits do not normalize over time and children
do not become resilient to iPad and media use effects. This finding will be present even while
controlling for a number of potential confounding factors such as gender, race and ethnicity,
socioeconomic status, gestational age of child at birth, birth weight, maternal use of alcohol or
tobacco during pregnancy, language spoken in the home, amount of media use within the last
week, and starting age of media use. !
Two explanations support this predicted result. One explanation addresses children’s
amount of media use such that longer amounts of media use are associated with attention
problems in children (Landhuis, Poulton, Welch, & Hancox, 2007). Researchers predict that
children who use iPads are using media for overall longer amounts of time than those who watch
TV exclusively. Because iPads are portable, they can be taken and used in multiple settings.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
36!
They have become one of the most convenient behavioral regulation tools for parents, who will
allow their children to use mobile media in order to pacify them during routine outings and social
events (Radesky, Schumacher, & Zuckerman, 2015). Children thus have access to the iPad in
more contexts than TV such as during car rides, restaurant outings, and in waiting rooms, thereby
leading to longer amounts of use and exposure to media. In addition, when parents use mobile
media as a behavioral regulation tool, children will be less likely to develop sustained attention
on at-hand situations or tasks. Children will learn to rely on media devices for entertainment and
distraction instead of learning to sit still, self-regulate, and focus on their immediate
surroundings regardless of their appeal.
Another explanation for the strong association between iPad use and shorter attention
spans involves level of exogenous stimuli exposure. Researchers expect that in comparation to
TV use, iPad use will correlate with higher levels of exposure to exogenous stimuli. In contrast
to TV, the iPad has multiple modalities and interactive capabilities (Radesky, Schumacher, &
Zuckerman, 2015). With iPad use, children are exposed to continuously shifting and interactive
stimuli that drive and direct their attention. Because of the numerous exogenous interactive
stimuli and constantly changing visual stimulation in iPad apps and games, children may not
develop the ability to inhibit visual distracters, and thus face problems in the development of
focused visual spatial attention span in circumstances without rapid exogenous stimulation.
Notably, researchers hypothesize that interactive iPad use will correlate with the shortest visual
spatial attention spans, due to the highest levels of exogenous stimuli associated with interactive
media coupled with high levels of pervasiveness and longer amounts of use. !
This will be the first study to test the hypotheses of iPad use effects on visual spatial
attention span in early childhood. Because this study marks the start of research on iPad use
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
37!
effects on attention, a correlational study design is necessary to first establish an association
between shorter visual spatial attention span and iPad use. However, because of the correlational
design, causal inferences cannot be drawn from the observed associations. Frustrated parents
who want to keep their child occupied and calm may end up giveing their children who have
shorter visual spatial attention spans more access to mobile media. To mitigate this limitation,
researchers will exploit the longitudinally of the data set and control for the starting age of media
use to be 3 years old or younger for all participants. Attention span first begins to develop at 3
years old (Ruff & Capozzoli, 2003). Therefore, starting media use at this age or younger
increases the likelihood that participants will have similar initial attention spans, thus supporting
the hypothesis that iPad use has negative effects on attention. However, there are characteristics
associated with parenting styles and parents who allow their children to use excessive amounts of
media that could account for the relationship between iPad use and attention effects. For
example, more neglectful or preoccupied parents might allow their children to use more media.
This study is not designed to control for parenting style or domestic environment, and therefore
future research should include these measures in their analyses. !
Another limitation that warrants consideration is the parent-reported measure used for
amount of iPad use. Parental report of media use may not be an accurate measure of the true
amount of media use. Although prior research using parental report of television viewed has not
resulted in bias findings in either direction, future research could assess amount of media use
through more direct means such as through direct observation of participant viewing, daily time-
use diaries that also include the shows and apps actually used, or mobile media programs that
track and log iPad use (Christakis, Zimmerman, DiGiuseppe, & McCarty, 2004). However, even
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
38!
if the measurement is merely inaccurate, the effect of random measurement error is to bias the
results toward the null. !
A similar limitation would be in regards to the dependent variable, visual spatial attention
span, which will be measured using the RSVP subtest from the Early Childhood Attention
Battery. While previous research has found RSVP tasks to be accurate measurements of visual
spatial attention span, the test that children will take in this study is a brief instrument of
measurement (Breckenridge, Braddick, & Atkinson, 2013). A more thorough assessment of
outcomes including a variety of outcome measurements could reduce measurement error and
increase the ability to detect an effect. !
Despite these limitations, this study can have important implications. The existing
guidelines on media use recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics are established
solely on research on TV effects on childhood development (American Academy of Pediatrics,
2011). With the rise in young children’s mobile media use, knowing the effects of mobile media
and iPad use can help inform and update recommended guidelines for early childhood media use.
Parents, educators, caregivers, and pediatricians need to know how mobile media use should be
monitored and the potentially lasting implications of such use on their children’s cognitive
development. Researchers expect that this study will add visual spatial attention effects to the
previously studied consequences of excessive mobile media use, including poor socialization and
potentially poor educational achievement (Radesky, Schumacher, & Zuckerman, 2015;
Roseberry, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2014). However, because the first iPads were released in
2010, it is not possible for this study to examine whether such effects of mobile media use extend
into adolescence and beyond. Future research is needed to track cognitive and social implications
of mobile media use into adolescent development. A longitudinal study following mobile media
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
39!
users from early childhood into adolescence can directly track these individual developmental
changes over time, adding crucial research to the understanding of mobile media use effects.
Overall, while there may be many factors that influence attention span, the results from
this study could help parents and caregivers to take preventative action with respect to shorter
visual spatial attention span in children. Eliminating the use of mobile media as a behavior
regulation tool and setting time limits and reduced weekly limits to mobile media use can lower
children’s risk of developing shorter visual spatial attention spans. Because attention span is
crucial to a young child’s development and learning, any improvement that can be made to
children’s attention span is meaningful.
!
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
40!
References
American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Public Education. (2011). Media education.
Pediatrics, 128(5), 1040–1045. doi:104:341–343
Anderson, D.R., & Hanson, K.G. (2013). What researchers have learned about toddlers and
television. Zero to Three, 33(4), 4-10.
Anderson, D. R., & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young children. American
Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 505-522. doi:10.1177/0002764204271506
Breckenridge, K., Braddick, O., & Atkinson, J. (2013). The organization of attention in typical
development: A new preschool attention test battery. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 31(3), 271-288. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12004
Chica, A. B., Bartolomeo, P., & Lupiáñez, J. (2013). Two cognitive and neural systems for
endogenous and exogenous spatial attention. Behavioural Brain Research, 237107-123.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.09.027
Christakis, D.A. (2014). Interactive media use at younger than the age of 2 years: Time to
rethink the American academy of pediatrics guideline? Journal of the American Medical
Association Pediatrics, 168(5), 399-400.
Christakis D.A., Zimmerman F.J., DiGiuseppe, D.L., & McCarty C.A. (2004). Early television
exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children. Pediatrics, 113(4), 708–713.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.
Common Sense Media. (2013). Zero to eight: Children's media use in America. Retrieved from
http://www.commonsensemedia.org/research.
Foster, E. M., & Watkins, S. (2010). The value of reanalysis: TV viewing and attention
problems. Child Development, 81(1), 368-375.
doi:10.1111/j.14678624.2009.01400.x
Gola, A. H., & Calvert, S. L. (2011). Infants’ visual attention to baby dvds as a function of
program pacing. Infancy, 16(3), 295-305. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00051.x
Hornik, R. (1981). Out-of-school television and schooling: Hypotheses and methods. Review
of Educational Research, 51(2), 193-214. doi:10.3102/00346543051002193
Huston, A. C., & Wright, J. C. (1983). Children’s processing of television: The informative
functions of formal features. In J. Bryant & D. R. Anderson (Eds.), Children’s
understanding of television: Research on attention and comprehension (pp. 35–68). New
York, NY: Academic Press, Inc.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
41!
Kannass, K. N., Oakes, L. M., & Shaddy, D. J. (2006). A longitudinal investigation of the
development of attention and distractibility. Journal of Cognition and Development,
7(3), 381-409. doi:10.1207/s15327647jcd0703_8
Krcmar, M., & Cingel, D. P. (2014). Parent–child joint reading in traditional and electronic
formats. Media Psychology. doi:10.1080/15213269.2013.840243. [Epub ahead of print].
Kucirkova, N. (2014). IPads in early education: Separating assumptions and evidence. Front
Psychology, 715(5). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00715
Kumari, S., & Ahuja, S. (2010). Video viewing and cognitive development in preadolescents.
Social Science Computer Review, 28(2), 170-176. doi:10.1177/0894439309334815
Landhuis, C. E., Poulton, R., Welch, D., & Hancox, R. J. (2007). Does television viewing lead to
attention problems in adolescence? Results from a prospective longitudinal study.
Pediatrics, 120(3), 532-537. doi:10.1542/peds.2007-0978
Mahone, E. M., & Schneider, H. E. (2012). Assessment of attention in preschoolers.
Neuropsychology Review, 22(4), 361-383. doi:10.1007/s11065-012-9217-y
Oakes, L., Kannass, K., & Shaddy, D. (2002). Developmental changes in endogenous control
of attention: The role of target familiarity on infants’ distraction latency. Child
Development, 73, 1644–1655.
Posner, M.I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32,
3-25.
Radesky, J.S., Schumacher, J., & Zuckerman, B. (2015). Mobile and interactive media use by
young children: The good, the bad, and the unknown. Pediatrics, 135(1), 1-3.
doi:10.1542/peds.2014-2251
Radesky, J.S., Silverstein M., Zuckerman B., & Christakis D.A. (2014). Infant self-regulation
and early childhood media exposure. Pediatrics, 133(5), 1172-1178.
doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2367
Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Skype me! Socially contingent
interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85(3), 956-970.
doi:10.1111/cdev.12166
Ruff, H. A., & Capozzoli, M. C. (2003). Development of attention and distractibility in the
first 4 years of life. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 877-890.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.877
Singer, J.L. (1980). The power and limits of television: A cognitive-affective analysis. In P.
Tannenbaum (Ed.), The Entertainment Function of Television (pp. 312-360).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
42!
Swing, E. L., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., & Walsh, D. A. (2010). Television and video
game exposure and the development of attention problems. Pediatrics, 126(2), 214-221.
doi:10.1542/peds.2009-1508
Tablet ownership 2013 (2013). Pew Research Center Internet, Science, and Technology.
Retrieved from http://pewinternet.org/Reports/2013/Tablet-Ownership-2013.aspx
Wright, J. C., Huston, A. C., Ross, R., Calvert, S. L., Rollandeli, D., Weeks, L. A., … Potts, R.
(1984). Pace and continuity of television programs: Effects on children’s attention and
comprehension. Developmental Psychology, 20, 653–666.
Wright, J. C., & Vlietstra, A. (1975). The development of sustained attention: From
perceptual exploration to logical search. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child
development and behavior (pp. 195–239). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Zack, E., Barr, R., Gerhardstein, P., Dickerson, K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2009). Infant imitation
from television using novel touch screen technology. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 27(1), 13-26. doi:10.1348/026151008X334700
Zimmerman F.J., & Christakis D.A. (2005). Children’s television viewing and cognitive
outcomes: A longitudinal analysis of national data. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent
Medicine, 159(7), 619–625.
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
43!
Appendix A
RSVP Visual Spatial Attention Span Subtest Model
!
!
"#$%&'&(!)&*+#$,-.!/00!%(!
1-#2*3(#$%&'&(!$-#2*4+'.!5600!%(!
7+*82#!5!
7+*82#!/!
9!
Fixation(Point.!(,!:+*#$;$:+-#!<-,=(!
=>2*2!#,!',,<!+#!(#+*#9!?$(:'+@2)!A,*!B!
(2;,-)(!C2A,*2!(2D&2-;2!C28$-(9!
E3F!)$(#*+;#,*(!
E3F!)$(#*+;#,*(!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
44!
Appendix B
Pervasiveness of Media Use Measure
Please answer the following questions in regards to your child’s media use. 1= never 4=always
For iPad-users:
a. How often is the iPad used outside the home?
1 2 3 4
b. How often is the iPad used in the car?
1 2 3 4
c. How often is the iPad used when eating out in restaurant?
1 2 3 4
d. How often is the iPad used during social gatherings?
1 2 3 4
e. How often is the iPad used while waiting for services?
1 2 3 4
For TV-users
a. How often is TV viewed outside the home?
1 2 3 4
b. How often is TV viewed in the car?
1 2 3 4
c. How often is TV viewed when eating out in restaurant?
1 2 3 4
d. How often is TV viewed during social gatherings?
1 2 3 4
e. How often is TV viewed while waiting for services?
1 2 3 4
EARLY!CHILDHOOD!IPAD!USE!AND!EFFECTS!ON!ATTENTION!
45!
Appendix C
Demographics Survey
1. Select your child’s gender:
a. Male
b. Female
2. Select your child’s race/ethnicity:
a. White (non-Latino)
b. Latino
c. Asian/Asian American
d. Pacific Islander
e. African American/Black
f. Native American/Alaskan Native
g. Mixed
3. Enter your average annual household income:
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!_________________!
4. Enter the gestational age of your child at birth in months:
__________________!months
5. Enter the birth weight of your child in pounds to the nearest ounce
_____________ lbs. ____________oz.
!!
6. Did the mother use alcohol or tobacco at any time during her pregnancy?
a. Yes
b. No
7. What is the language spoken in the home?
a. English
b. Non-English
8. In the past week, how many hours did your child view TV/use the iPad?
_______________ hours
9. At what age, to the nearest year, did your child start viewing TV/using the iPad?
_______________ years old
!