Heads Up! Psychological Science
2011
Identifying Factors That Increase the Likelihood for Alcohol-Identifying Factors That Increase the Likelihood for Alcohol-
Induced Blackouts in the Prepartying Context Induced Blackouts in the Prepartying Context
Joseph W. LaBrie
Loyola Marymount University
Justin F. Hummer
Loyola Marymount University
Shannon Kenney
Loyola Marymount University
Andrew Lac
Loyola Marymount University
Eric Pedersen
University of Washington
Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lmu.edu/headsup
Part of the Psychology Commons
Repository Citation Repository Citation
LaBrie, Joseph W.; Hummer, Justin F.; Kenney, Shannon; Lac, Andrew; and Pedersen, Eric, "Identifying
Factors That Increase the Likelihood for Alcohol-Induced Blackouts in the Prepartying Context" (2011).
Heads Up!
. 21.
https://digitalcommons.lmu.edu/headsup/21
This Article - post-print is brought to you for free and open access by the Psychological Science at Digital
Commons @ Loyola Marymount University and Loyola Law School. It has been accepted for inclusion in Heads Up!
by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons@Loyola Marymount University and Loyola Law School. For
more information, please contact [email protected].
Identifying Factors That Increase the Likelihood for Alcohol-
Induced Blackouts in the Prepartying Context
Joseph W. LaBrie
1
, Justin Hummer
2
, Shannon Kenney
2
, Andrew Lac
2
, and Eric Pedersen
3
1
Department of Psychology, Loyola Marymount University, Los Angeles, USA
2
Department of Psychology, Loyola Marymount University, Los Angeles, USA
3
Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, USA
Abstract
The present study examined risk factors related to “blacking out” (e.g., temporary periods of
memory loss during drinking) during preparty drinking events (i.e., pregaming, predrinking).
Participants were students from two universities on the West Coast who reported past month
prepartying (N = 2,546) in online surveys administered in the fall of 2008. Among these students,
25% (n = 636) reported blacking out during at least one occasion in which they prepartied in the
past month. A logistic regression model underscored that Greek student affiliation, family history
of alcohol abuse, frequency of prepartying, and both playing drinking games and consuming shots
of liquor while prepartying increased the likelihood of blacking out. Limitations and implications
for future research and collegiate prevention strategies are discussed.
Keywords
alcohol; prepartying; pregaming; alcohol-induced; blackout; college students
INTRODUCTION
College students remain an at-risk population for heavy alcohol use and resulting
consequences including emotional, academic, physical, sexual, and legal problems
(Hingson, Zha, & Weitzman, 2009; Muraven, Collins, Morsheimer, Shiffman, & Paty, 2005;
Wechsler & Nelson, 2006). “Blackout” or “blacking out” refers to a period of time during a
drinking event in which an individual cannot recall all or parts of the event. These alcohol-
induced memory blackouts, also known as anterograde amnesia or acute alcohol-induced
memory dysfunction, may be particularly hazardous. During alcohol-induced blackouts, the
hippocampus, a region of the brain fundamental to memory function, is impaired,
subsequently causing cognitive deficiencies in transferring information from short-term to
long-term memory (Goodwin, 1995; White, 2003). In a blackout situation, individuals suffer
Copyright © 2011 Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.
Address correspondence to Dr. Joseph W. LaBriem, Department of Psychology, Loyola Marymount University, 1 LMU Drive, Los
Angeles, 90045; [email protected].
Declaration of Interest
The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the article.
NIH Public Access
Author Manuscript
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
Published in final edited form as:
Subst Use Misuse. 2011 ; 46(8): 992–1002. doi:10.3109/10826084.2010.542229.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
partial (i.e., fragmentary blackout) or complete (i.e., en bloc blackout) memory loss for
drinking events for which they were conscious and active. In addition to potential
neurocognitive impairment, which is especially problematic in younger populations still in
the midst of progressive neurodevelopment (Acheson, Stein, & Swartzwelder, 1998;
DeBellis et al., 2002; Zeigler et al., 2005), alcohol-induced blackouts also amplify proximal
risk (e.g., unsafe sexual behavior, driving while intoxicated, and violence).
Alcohol-Induced Blackouts
In college-based studies, students experiencing alcohol-induced blackouts have been found
to exhibit peak blood alcohol concentrations (BACs) of 0.21 to 0.35 and consume between
8.2 and 11.5 drinks for males and 3.7 and 5.0 drinks for females (Buelow & Koeppel, 2001;
Hartzler & Fromme, 2003; Hunt, 1993). In a study of more than 14,000 students from 119
colleges, 10% of nonbinge drinkers (the authors used the term “binge” to refer to heavy
episodic drinking events—5 or more drinks in a row for men or 4 or more drinks in a row
for women), 27% of occasional binge drinkers (1–2 times in past 2 weeks), and 54% of
frequent binge drinkers (3 or more times in past 2 weeks) reporting having blacked out
during the current school year (Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, & Lee, 2000). Although the link
between “heavy drinking” and blacking out is well known, the direct causes of blacking out
are fast-paced alcohol consumption and rapid elevation of BACs, which when combined are
speculated to overwhelm sensorimotor processes (Goodwin, Othmer, & Halikas, 1970;
Ryback, 1970; White, Jamieson-Drake, & Swartzwelder, 2002; White, Signer, Kraus, &
Swartzwelder, 2004). Whether due to alcohol naivety, underage drinking, the desire to
obtain a “buzz” before going out, or other factors such as playing consumption-focused
games with others, alcohol use in college often takes place away from authority figures
and/or legal drinking environments, which may lend itself to quick-paced drinking practices.
Therefore, it is not entirely surprising that blackouts are prevalent in college populations.
Other studies report that half of college student drinkers have experienced at least one
alcohol blackout in their lifetimes, 40% have blacked out in the past year, and 9.4% of those
who drank in the past 2 weeks have reported blacking out during the same period (Buelow,
1990; Hartzler & Fromme, 2003; White et al., 2002). Because individuals suffering
blackouts most often rely on others to cue or recount such events, assessing blackout-
associated risk is challenging. Nevertheless, adverse behavioral consequences include
vandalism, unsafe sex, drug use, driving under the influence, and fighting, as well as
stressful psychological consequences such as intrusive thoughts or sleep difficulties (Buelow
& Koeppel, 2001; White et al., 2002, 2004). In a sample of 50 undergraduate students who
reported a blackout and only later became aware of consequences, 50% of males and 38% of
females realized they had engaged in sexual activity with individuals they did not know,
38% of males and females had an argument or fight, 25% of males had vandalized property,
and 12.5% of males had driven a motor vehicle (White et al., 2004).
Prepartying Behavior in the College Context
Because blackouts most often result from quickly rising blood alcohol levels resulting from
fast-paced drinking, it is important to examine the type of drinking behaviors that can lead to
these dangerous states. Prepartying is a high-risk drinking behavior common among college
students that is receiving growing attention in the research literature. Also referred to as
LaBrie et al.
Page 2
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
“pregaming,” “preloading,” “front-loading,” “predrinking,” or “prefunking” (drinking before
a function), depending on the region or local group vernacular, prepartying involves “the
consumption of alcohol prior to attending an event or activity (e.g., party, bar, concert) at
which more alcohol may or may not be consumed” (Pedersen & LaBrie, 2007, p. 238).
Prepartying typically involves fast-paced drinking within brief periods of time, as an
inherent purpose is to create a “buzz” or level of inebriation that will heighten enjoyment of
the event and possibly endure through the event or until more alcohol can be obtained. This
style of drinking makes self-regulation appreciably more difficult and negative
consequences more likely. Prepartying has been found to predict numerous consequences
among college students such as academic neglect, hangovers, passing out, and fighting
(Pedersen & LaBrie, 2007; Pedersen, LaBrie, & Kilmer, 2009).
In three separate studies, researchers estimated that participants reached blood alcohol levels
near or above the legal intoxication limit (0.08) during actual prepartying drinking events
(LaBrie & Pedersen, 2008; Pedersen & LaBrie, 2007; Pedersen et al., 2009). For the
majority of participants, this blood alcohol level was achieved prior to going out and
consuming even more alcohol. In event-level studies, 80% of preparty drinking events
involved further drinking (Pedersen & LaBrie, 2007), and when including drinks consumed
during and after prepartying, students achieved mean blood alcohol levels of 0.15 on
prepartying days (LaBrie & Pedersen, 2008). This elevated level of intoxication is
immensely risk enhancing, significantly impairing motor control, vision, and decision-
making. Thus, prepartying may be especially linked to blackouts. Understanding the link
between prepartying and blacking out among college students will prove helpful both to
researchers who seek to understand college student drinking and practitioners seeking to
design and implement interventions that reduce the risk associated with both of these events.
Study Aims and Hypotheses
The present study examined a large representative sample of students reporting prepartying
behavior to determine how common blacking out was during prepartying events and
whether differences emerged between those who experienced preparty-related blackouts and
those who did not. For this purpose, the primary analysis entailed a binary logistic
regression model of demographic characteristics (age, gender, race, Greek status, family
history of alcohol abuse), prepartying frequency, method of prepartying (alone, with friends/
roommates, while playing drinking games), and type of alcoholic beverage consumed during
prepartying (shots, wine, and mixed drinks) as potential predictors that increased the
likelihood of a blackout. Specifically, it was anticipated that blacking out would be fairly
common among prepartying college students and that risk factors such as prepartying
frequency, playing drinking games while prepartying, and drinking shots of liquor would
greatly contribute to blacking out.
METHOD
Participants
Recruitment and data collection occurred at two West Coast campuses, one a large public
university and the other a private mid-sized university, in 2008. The study was approved by
LaBrie et al.
Page 3
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
the Human Subjects Review Boards at both participating universities. Data were collected as
part of a larger longitudinal intervention study, and all data used in analyses were derived
from the screening survey that was completed before administration of any intervention. Of
the 11,069 students invited to participate in the study, 4,984 (45.0%) completed the initial
survey. Only those 2,546 (51.1%) participants who answered with a value of at least 1 to the
open-ended question, “In the last 30 days, how many days did you engage in prepartying?”
received further preparty questions and were therefore included in the final analyses.
Demographic characteristics of this prepartying sample revealed that they were primarily
female (58.6%) and Caucasian (57.1%). Other ethnic and racial representations were as
follows: 20.2% Asian American, 11.0% Hispanic, 2.1% African American, and 9.6% other/
multiracial. Review of demographic information from the registrar’s office at both
universities revealed that the percentages of women and Caucasians represented in the
sample were comparable with the overall compositions of the general student populations.
Minor discrepancies in the representativeness of ethnic minorities were present for each
institution.
Design and Procedure
At the end of the first month of the fall term, students were randomly selected from registrar
rosters for recruitment. Students received both an email and a postal delivered invitation to
participate in a study regarding alcohol use. If the student chose to participate, he/she
clicked on a link to the online survey, entered a unique Personal Identification Number
(PIN) and was prompted to electronically consent to the study before being directed to the
20-minute screening survey itself. Each student received a nominal stipend of $15 to
complete the survey. The survey assessed demographic variables, as well as several
variables related to alcohol use. Prior to questions assessing drinking behavior, one standard
drink was defined for students as one-half ounce (oz) of ethyl alcohol, one 12 oz beer, one 4
oz glass of wine, or one 1.25 oz. shot. Pictures of standard drinks accompanied these
descriptions.
Measures
Demographics—Background characteristics measured included age, gender, and race.
Also assessed were Greek status (“Are you a member of a fraternity or sorority?”) and
family history of alcohol abuse
1
(“To your knowledge, do you have any biological relatives
that have a significant drinking problem—one that should or did lead to treatment?”).
Prepartying Behavior—Prior to questions assessing prepartying, the behavior was
defined as “the consumption of alcohol prior to attending an event or activity [e.g., party,
bar, concert] at which more alcohol may or may not be consumed” (Pedersen & LaBrie,
2007, p. 238). Participants first responded to the following open-ended question: “In the last
30 days, how many days did you engage in prepartying?” When participants indicated at
least one prepartying day, they were then asked, “On average, how many drinks did you
consume while prepartying (not including drinks consumed after arriving to your planned
1
The journal’s style utilizes the category substance abuse as a diagnostic category. Substances are used or misused; living organisms
are and can be abused. Editor’s note.
LaBrie et al. Page 4
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
destination)?” This was followed by the question, “How did you typically preparty?”
Respondents were asked to check all that apply: (1) “Alone, while getting ready to go out”
(prepartying alone); (2) “With friends/roommates, while getting ready to go out”
(prepartying with friends/roommates); and (3) “Playing drinking games” (prepartying while
playing drinking games). The next question assessed type of alcoholic beverage consumed
while prepartying: “What did you typically drink while prepartying?” Again, respondents
were asked to check all that apply: (1) “beer”; (2) “shots”; (3) “wine”; and (4) “mixed
drinks. ” Finally, participants reported on the following question representing the dependent
variable of interest: “How many times in the past month (30 days) did you blackout on a
night when you prepartied? If this never happened to you, please leave the question blank or
type ‘0’.” This item was on an open-ended response scale and indicated the number of times
the participant experienced a blackout during or after a prepartying event.
Typical Alcohol Use Behavior—Quantity of alcohol consumption was assessed using
the Daily Drinking Questionnaire (DDQ; Collins, Parks, & Marlatt, 1985). Participants were
asked, “Consider a typical week in the last month. How much alcohol, on average (measured
in number of drinks), did you drink on each day of a typical week?” Participants responded
by reporting the typical number of drinks consumed on each day of the week. Weekly
drinking was calculated by summing participants’ responses for each day of the week. The
DDQ has been used in previous studies of college student drinking and has demonstrated
good validity (
Larimer et al., 2001; Marlatt et al., 1998).
RESULTS
Data Analysis
The dependent variable assessed the number of times in the past 30 days participants
blacked out on a night when they engaged in prepartying. Owing to some respondents
reporting extremely high values, this open-ended response variable displayed highly non-
normal distributional properties (skewness = 8.00, p < .001; kurtosis = 137.80, p < .001).
Thus, it was necessary to binary code the outcome into whether a respondent experienced a
blacked out at least once (1) or never (0). For the purpose of interpretation in a logistic
regression model, the predictor variables of respondent age and prepartying days in the past
30 days each were recoded and analyzed as discrete categorical variables (see Table 1 for
categories), enabling the comparison levels of a variable to be contrasted to its respective
reference level (Norusis, 2003).
Initial data analyses examined information with regard to number of prepartying days,
prepartying drinks, typical alcohol use behavior, whether participants blacked out, and
gender differences. Next, chi-square tests determined significant bivariate relationships
between each of the 13 categorical variables (see Table 1 for variables) and whether
respondents had experienced a blackout on a prepartying night in the past 30 days. Finally, a
more comprehensive analysis collectively incorporated all variables into a binary logistic
regression model predicting the experience of having a blackout on a prepartying night.
Results from this analysis would yield valuable insight into the most important risk factors
contributing to whether respondents experienced a black out, after statistically controlling
LaBrie et al.
Page 5
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
for, and therefore ruling out, all other covariates in the model. The binary logistic regression
model was specified and estimated according to established procedures (Tabachnick &
Fidell, 2007). The parameter of interest assessing the contribution of each predictor in this
statistical technique is the odds ratio (OR). A null hypothesis OR of 1.0 represents no
difference in ratio of proportion of blacking out (vs. not blacking out) between a specific
comparison level (e.g., Asian, Hispanic, African American, and other/multiracial) and its
corresponding reference level (e.g., Caucasian). The OR also serves as an indicator of effect
size (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
Initial Analyses
Participants reported the following number of days they prepartied in the past 30 days: 1
(19.2%), 2–5 (52.1%), 6–9 (17.9%), and 10+(10.8%). Among male participants, the number
of prepartying days reported were 1 (16.4%), 2–5 (50.0%), 6–9 (20.2%), and 10+ (13.4%).
Among female participants, the number of prepartying days reported were 1 (21.1%), 2–5
(53.6%), 6–9 (16.2%), and 10+ (9.0%). A 2 (gender) × 4 (prepartying days categories)
analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on the dependent variable of average number
of drinks consumed during a typical preparty occasion. Results revealed a main effect for
both gender, F (1, 2,538) = 186.74, p < .001, and prepartying days categories, F (3, 2,538) =
136.45, p < .001, in addition to their interaction effect, F (3, 2,538) = 8.39, p < .001.
Representation of the interaction effect in Figure 1 shows that males averaged more
beverages during a typical prepartying occasion than females, and that this gender
discrepancy became especially pronounced as the number of prepartying days increased.
Descriptive data also indicated that 25.0% (n = 636) of this prepartying sample of
respondents experienced a blackout in the past 30 days on a night when they prepartied, with
14.2% (n = 361) blacking out once, 5.7% (n = 145) blacking out twice, and 5.1% (n = 130)
blacking out three or more times. In terms of total drinks per week from the DDQ, males
who experienced a blackout consumed 23.14 (standard deviation [SD] = 13.03) drinks per
week compared with 11.85 (SD = .95) drinks per week among males not reporting a
blackout, t (1,052) = 15.41, p < .001. Females reporting a blackout consumed an average of
12.77 (SD = 7.76) total drinks per week in contrast to 6.91 (SD = 5.88) drinks among
females not reporting a blackout, t (1,492) = 14.99, p < .001.
Furthermore, we examined the average number of prepartying drinks consumed during a
typical prepartying event (not including drinks consumed after prepartying). Among the
overall sample, 34.9% of males and 29.4% of females reported engaging in heavy episodic
drinking during a typical prepartying event (consuming 4+ drinks for women and 5+ drinks
for men in a row; O’Malley & Johnston, 2002; Wechsler & Nelson, 2008). Specifically,
among those who reported blacking out, 56.0% of males and 49.7% of females engaged in
heavy episodic drinking during a typical prepartying event. However, among those not
reporting a blackout, only 26.6% of males and 23.2% of females engaged in heavy episodic
drinking during a typical prepartying event. In addition, males who reported blacking out
consumed an average of 4.96 (SD = 2.02) drinks during a typical prepartying event,
compared with 3.67 (SD = 2.02) drinks for males who did not report blacking out, t (1,056)
= 9.29, p < .001. Females who experienced a blackout averaged 3.66 (SD = 1.42) drinks
LaBrie et al.
Page 6
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
during a typical prepartying event, in comparison with 2.70 (SD=1.48) drinks for females
not experiencing a blackout, t (1,493) = 10.76, p < .001.
Variables Bivariately Associated With Blacking Out
Results of the chi-square tests of independence between each variable and blacking out are
displayed in Table 1. Ten of the 13 variables were evidenced to be bivariately associated
with blacking out. Respondents who blacked out, in comparison to those who did not,
tended to be male, Caucasian, Greek-affiliated, and possessed a family history of alcohol
misuse. In addition to these demographic factors, the cohort experiencing the blackout
tended to engage in more days of prepartying, preparty while playing drinking games, as
well as be more likely to consume beer, shots, wine, or mixed drinks during prepartying
events. Age, prepartying alone, and prepartying with friends/roommates did not emerge as
factors associated with blacking out.
Predictive Model
Results from the binary logistic regression model predicting blacking out are presented in
Table 2 and interpreted after statistically controlling for the variance explicated by all other
predictors entered into the model. The overall logistic regression model, with all predictors
entered, emerged as highly significant, Nagelkerke R
2
= 23.7%, χ
2
(18) = 442.83, p < .001.
Specifically, Asian (OR = 0.73, p < .05) and African American (OR = 0.43, p < .05) students
tended to be less likely to blackout than their Caucasian peers. Greek status (OR = 1.37, p
< .01) and family history of alcohol misuse (1.24, p < .05) also served as significant
predictors of blacking out. In contrast to respondents engaging in only 1 day of prepartying
in the past 30 days, respondents prepartying 2–5 days (OR = 6.36, p < .001), 6–9 days (OR
= 14.73, p < .001), and 10 or more days (OR = 27.01, p < .001) experienced elevated, and
extremely high, rates of suffering a blackout. Increased risk of experiencing a blackout was
also predicted by prepartying while playing drinking games (OR = 1.26, p < .05) and
prepartying while consuming shots (OR = 1.60, p < .001).
DISCUSSION
The current study assessed the prevalence of past month prepartying behavior and
particularly the concomitant risk of blacking out during a prepartying event in a large
sample of college students. The study further evaluated potential characteristics associated
with those who did or did not experience a blackout on a prepartying night in the past month
and the factors that contributed to the likelihood of a prepartying event resulting in a
blackout. Findings indicated that both prepartying and blacking out when prepartying were
common, with over half of the participants reporting prepartying during the past month and,
among these, one fourth reporting blacking out during a prepartying event. Those who
blacked out drank at higher levels during prepartying than those who did not blackout.
Although typical hours spent consuming beverages was not assessed, previous research
looking at multiple prepartying events revealed that approximately 50% of students’
prepartying events lasted less than 1 hour, with approximately 90% lasting less than 2 hours
(
Pedersen & LaBrie, 2007). This is consistent with research documenting the relationship
between blackouts and a rapid rate of alcohol consumption (White et al., 2002). Descriptive
LaBrie et al.
Page 7
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
data on the current sample revealed that compared with prepartiers who did not report
blacking out during the past month, male and female students who did report a blackout
drank approximately twice as much during a typical week and consumed more drinks during
a typical prepartying occasion. Further, approximately 56% of males and 50% of females
who had experienced a blackout while prepartying engaged in heavy episodic drinking
during a typical prepartying event compared with only 27% of males and 23% of females
who did not experience a blackout. Although the actual duration of drinking events and
subsequent timing of the ensuing blackout was not directly assessed in this study, fast-paced
drinking during prepartying (Pedersen&LaBrie, 2007) is likely associated with reductions in
judgment and minimization of potential consequences (Fromme, Katz, & D’Amico, 1997),
whereby leading to heavier drinking following the prepartying event later in the night.
Indeed, the amount of alcohol consumed during prepartying directly predicts the amount
consumed afterwards (Pedersen & LaBrie, 2007). These links further categorize prepartying
as a risky behavior with the potential for quickly elevated BACs leading to blacking out and
potentially dangerous negative consequences associated with blacking out.
Chi-square tests revealed bivariate relationships between blacking out and the following
factors: being male, being Caucasian, Greek status, family history of alcohol abuse,
prepartying more frequently, prepartying while playing drinking games, and, not
surprisingly, consuming larger amounts of alcohol during prepartying, whether it be beer,
wine, shots, or mixed drinks. Of notable interest is that age did not play a role in
discriminating among students who reported blacking out while prepartying. Although the
genesis of prepartying is not known, one possible speculation is that it emerged from
underage drinkers (those under the legal drinking age of 21 in the United States) not having
access to alcohol once they go out for the evening. For example, underage students may
consume alcohol prior to attending a club that requires students be 18 years of age to enter
but 21 or older to consume alcohol. In addition, concert venues, sporting arenas and
stadiums, or school-sponsored events often are either alcohol free or require that students be
of legal drinking age in order to consume alcoholic beverages. This may be why in one
study that specifically focused on reasons for prepartying among a young college-aged
population, males and females reported arriving to a social event already under the influence
as their most highly endorsed reason for prepartying (Pedersen et al., 2009). Yet while
prepartying is popular among underage drinkers seeking intoxication whether it be before a
school-related function, party, social, or sporting event, it also remains popular among
students of legal drinking age. Previous studies have found no differences between underage
and of-age participants on the number of drinks consumed while prepartying or the
frequency of prepartying in the past month (e.g., Glindemann, Ehrhart, Maynard, & Geller,
2006; Pedersen et al., 2009). Coupled with the current results, it appears that risky drinking
behaviors, such as prepartying, may need to be demythologized as a primarily first-year
student or underage drinking phenomenon.
Previous research has identified factors such as being Caucasian (Office of Applied Studies,
2008; O’Malley & Johnston, 2002), Greek status (Larimer, Anderson, Baer, & Marlatt,
2000; Park, Sher, & Krull, 2008), possessing a family history of alcohol misuse (Warner,
White, & Johnson, 2007), drinking frequently (Borsari, Neal, Collins, & Carey, 2001), and
playing drinking games (Borsari et al., 2007) to be associated with risky or problematic
LaBrie et al.
Page 8
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
drinking. Thus, for a more comprehensive examination, we incorporated all potential factors
into a logistic regression model to determine which factors uniquely accounted for blacking
out while prepartying when controlling for the other factors. Similar to previous findings,
the likelihood of having had a blackout was uniquely predicted by being in a fraternity or
sorority, possessing a family history of alcohol abuse, engaging in prepartying more
frequently, playing drinking games while prepartying, and consuming shots of liquor while
prepartying.
Identifying prepartying factors that provide unique risk for preparty-related blackouts
provides information that can be incorporated into targeted prevention efforts. Prevention
and intervention messaging could discuss the dangers of combining drinking games with
prepartying, as well as the heightened risk associated with drinking shots of liquor in the
context of prepartying. White et al. (2004) also found that students were more likely to
blackout on nights in which they were drinking liquor. This is likely due to the quickened
drinking and absorption factors typically associated with liquor as opposed to other types of
alcoholic beverages such as beer or wine. Furthermore, drinking games have been linked to
numerous consequences in college students (e.g., hangovers, campus violations, reliance on
alcohol, risky sexual behaviors, and car accidents; Borsari et al., 2007; Pedersen & LaBrie,
2006). Problems may be especially compounded in the context of prepartying, however,
given the short duration and the possibility that some students may be unfamiliar with the
acute effects of alcohol. Thus, when consuming several shots or engaging in drinking games
within the short period of the “preparty,” students may not realize the intoxication levels that
will be reached following time-delayed absorption, which may account for the dangerously
high prevalence of blacking out.
Further, previous research has found that female students are more likely to consume shots
of liquor when prepartying than male students, who typically drink beer when prepartying
(Pedersen & LaBrie, 2007). Also, women experienced more alcohol-related consequences
on a prepartying day than on a non-prepartying day (LaBrie & Pedersen, 2008). Thus, the
current findings showing that drinking shots of liquor when prepartying increases the
likelihood of blacking out hold particular relevance for women’s health and well-being.
Women absorb alcohol more quickly than men, reaching higher BACs more rapidly and,
when blacked out or at risky BACs, are at risk for serious consequences, particularly
sexually-related negative consequences. Focusing on prepartying behaviors among women,
while providing information on the physiological differences between men and women in
processing alcohol, may be particularly effective at reducing risk among female students.
Finally, blacking out has traditionally been linked to dosages commonly referred to as
“acute excessive alcohol consumption,” “fast-paced drinking,” “high blood alcohol levels,”
or “heavy episodic drinking.” Such descriptions may not resonate with students when
promoting prevention or intervention messages. Prepartying (and its regional variations),
however, is identifiable as a behavior well established in collegiate nomenclature and that
can be quantifiably researched. As a result, prepartying may hold more potential as a target
for efficacious experimental manipulation than other, more abstract, concepts of “heavy
drinking.”
LaBrie et al.
Page 9
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Study Limitations and Future Directions
The current study has several limitations. First, this study did not specify or define the type
of blackout that was experienced. It is possible that referring to a generic term of “blacking
out” may have resulted in students thinking about times they woke up with no memories,
also known as en bloc. However, research suggests that fragmentary blackouts are most
common among college students (White et al., 2004), and thus participants may actually
have underreported the frequency of blackout occurrence while prepartying. Similarly, the
wording of the question may have led students to answer as if the blackout referred to the
prepartying event itself; however, this is unlikely given that the text read as “… did you
blackout on a night when you prepartied.” Anecdotal experiences during our work with
students suggested that our phrasing of the question was understood to indicate that the
blackout could have occurred at any point during the night (i.e., during the prepartying event
or after). Second, while the generalizability of the participants within each site was indicated
by the representative percentage of female and Caucasian participants, the slightly
discrepant percentages of African American, Asian American, and Hispanic American
participants may have led to inconclusive findings when comparing Caucasians with these
groups. Studies with ample number of minority participants specifically targeting
differences in ethnicity-specific prepartying behavior are needed. Third, all measures were
self-report and therefore subject to errors in recall. However, this limitation is attenuated by
the fact that participants were repeatedly assured of confidentiality and that blacking out is a
salient event for most students, meaning that they may be more apt to accurately recall
specific characteristics surrounding prepartying events. As length of time typically spent
prepartying was not assessed, we were unable to determine if participants generally reached
blackout BACs (Buelow & Koeppel, 2001; Hartzler & Fromme, 2003; Hunt, 1993) during
prepartying or if prepartying associated with increased risk for further drinking that led to a
blackout.
While this study focused on the blackout as the measure of consequence, it would also be
interesting to garner a sense of the potential consequences that blacking out may have
caused. Future research could include a wider assessment of whether students were able to
find out what transpired while they were blacked out and if any physical, psychological, or
psychosocial consequence occurred as a result. Circulating such data to other students could
aid in creating awareness of psychological stressors that could potentially occur in the
aftermath of a blackout, prompting reconsideration of engaging in future “heavy drinking”
episodes. Finally, recent research has documented a high (45%) prevalence rate of
prepartying during the final months of high school, which was subsequently found to
prospectively increase alcohol-related risk upon the transition into college (Kenney,
Hummer, & LaBrie, 2010). In light of the current findings, research should continue to focus
on high school and noncollege samples to determine whether memory blackouts or other
serious consequences are associated with prepartying in drinking environments other than
college. Novel and tailored intervention approaches for this risky drinking context are
clearly needed and such calls are being increasingly echoed in recent research (e.g., Read,
Merrill, & Bytschkow, 2010).
LaBrie et al.
Page 10
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Summary
Prepartying is a high-risk drinking context that is common in college students and which
often leads to blacking out. By examining factors that predispose students to hazardous
scenarios, practitioners can potentially make a stronger impact in reducing some of these
risks via intervention and prevention efforts. The present study began to address some
specific indicators involved in the likelihood of blacking out while prepartying, including
drinking shots of liquor, playing drinking games, prepartying frequently, and racial/ethnic
identity. Thus, college student affairs personnel are encouraged to remain cognizant of these
markers and create novel messaging and programming targeting these risk-inducing factors.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by grant R01 AA 012547–06A2 from the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and
Alcoholism and grant Q184H070017 from the US Department of Education.
Biographies
Joseph W. LaBrie obtained a Ph.D. in clinical psychology in 2002 from the University of
Southern California, in addition to holding a M.Div. in theology and a M.S. in mathematics.
He is currently the Special Assistant to the President, Associate Professor of psychology,
and Director of the Heads Up research lab at the Loyola Marymount University. His
research interests are focused on prevention and intervention efforts for risky behaviors
among young adults and adolescents. Dr. LaBrie has published over 70 research articles in
this area as well and been the recipient of numerous private and federal grants to study
young adult health behaviors and approaches to prevention and intervention.
Justin F. Hummer is the Assistant Director of the Heads Up research lab at the Loyola
Marymount University. His primary research interests consider how social and motivational
factors relate to the etiology, prevention, and treatment of health-risk behaviors among
college students.
LaBrie et al.
Page 11
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Shannon R. Kenney graduated from the Brown University in 2006 with a Ph.D. in
sociology. She is currently a postdoctoral fellow in the Heads Up research lab and Visiting
Assistant Research Professor at the Loyola Marymount University. Her research has focused
on youth and adolescent health, including the treatment and prevention of substance misuse
and poorer mental health.
Andrew Lac is earning his Ph.D. in psychology at the Claremont Graduate University, and
serves as a Statistical Consultant at the Loyola Marymount University and the University of
Southern California. With interests encompassing social, developmental, and health
psychology, his published research applies multivariate methods to examine adolescent
delinquency and family dynamics.
Eric R. Pedersen is a doctoral candidate in the clinical psychology program at the
University of Washington. His interests are in young adult substance use. In particular, he is
interested in exploring the contextual factors related to alcohol and marijuana use among
college students.
GLOSSARY
Alcohol-Induced
Blackout
Period of time in which memory is impaired by alcohol consumption
such that an individual is unable to recall all or parts of a past
drinking event (also known as anterograde amnesia or acute alcohol-
induced memory dysfunction).
En Bloc
Blackout
A blackout in which an individual suffers complete memory loss
related to a drinking event, even when provided details.
Fragmentary
Blackout
A blackout associated with partial memory loss in which an
individual is able to recall some but not all aspects of a drinking
event, and is often able to remember details when prompted.
Prepartying
Also referred to as pregaming, preloading, front-loading, predrinking,
or prefunking, prepartying involves the consumption of alcohol prior
to attending an event or activity (e.g., party, bar, concert) at which
more alcohol may or may not be consumed.
LaBrie et al. Page 12
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
REFERENCES
Acheson S, Stein R, Swartzwelder H. Impairment of semantic and figural memory by acute ethanol:
Age-dependent effects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1998; 22(7):1437–1442.
Borsari B, Boyle KE, Hustad JTP, Barnett NP, Tevyaw TO, Kahler CW. Drinking before drinking:
Pregaming and drinking games in mandated students. Addictive Behaviors. 2007; 32(11):2694–
2705. [PubMed: 17574344]
Borsari B, Neal DJ, Collins SE, Carey KB. Differential utility of three indexes of risky drinking for
predicting alcohol problems in college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2001; 15(4):
321–324. [PubMed: 11767264]
Buelow, GB. An analysis of the influence of dysfunctional roles, chemical dependence, and family
functioning on college adjustment. Eugene: Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Oregon; 1990.
Buelow G, Koeppel J. Psychological consequences of alcohol induced blackout among college
students. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 2001; 40(3):10–20.
Collins RL, Parks GA, Marlatt GA. Social determinants of alcohol consumption: The effects of social
interaction and model status on the self-administration of alcohol. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology. 1985; 53:189–200. [PubMed: 3998247]
DeBellis MD, Clark DB, Beers SR, Soloff PH, Boring AM, Hall J, et al. Hippocampal volume in
adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002; 157:737–744.
Fromme K, Katz E, D’Amico E. Effects of alcohol intoxication on the perceived consequences of risk
taking. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1997; 5(1):14–23. [PubMed: 9234035]
Glindemann KE, Ehrhart IJ, Maynard ML, Geller ES. Letter to the editor: Alcohol front-loading
among college students: Exploring the need for prevention intervention. Journal of Alcohol and
Drug Education. 2006; 50:5–13.
Goodwin D. Alcohol amnesia. Addiction. 1995; 90(3):315–317. [PubMed: 7735016]
Goodwin DW, Othmer E, Halikas JA. Loss of short-term memory as a predictor of the alcoholic
“black-out”. Nature. 1970; 227:201–202. [PubMed: 4913709]
Hartzler B, Fromme K. Fragmentary and en bloc blackouts: Similarity and distinction among episodes
of alcohol-induced memory loss. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2003; 64(4):547–550. [PubMed:
12921196]
Hingson RW, Zha W, Weitzman ER. Magnitude of and trends in alcohol-related mortality and
morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24, 1998–2005. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and
Drugs. 2009 Jul.(Suppl. 16):12–20. [PubMed: 19538908]
Hunt W. Are binge drinkers more at risk of developing brain damage? Alcohol. 1993; 10(6):559–561.
[PubMed: 8123218]
Kenney SR, Hummer JF, LaBrie JW. An examination of prepartying and drinking game playing
during high school and their impact on alcohol-related risk upon entrance into college. Journal of
Youth and Adolescence. 2010; 39:999–1011. [PubMed: 19904593]
LaBrie JW, Pedersen ER. Prepartying promotes heightened risk in the college environment: An event-
level report. Addictive Behaviors. 2008; 33:955–959. [PubMed: 18387749]
Larimer M, Anderson B, Baer J, Marlatt G. An individual in context: Predictors of alcohol use and
drinking problems among Greek and residence hall students. Journal of Substance Abuse. 2000;
11(1):53–68. [PubMed: 10756514]
Larimer ME, Turner AP, Anderson BK, Fader JS, Kilmer JR, Palmer RS, et al. Evaluating a brief
alcohol intervention with fraternities. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2001; 62(3):370–380.
[PubMed: 11414347]
Lipsey, MW.; Wilson, DB. Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2001.
Marlatt GA, Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Dimeff LA, Larimer ME, Quigley LA, et al. Screening and brief
intervention for high-risk college student drinkers: Results from a 2-year follow-up assessment.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1998; 66(4):604–615. [PubMed: 9735576]
LaBrie et al.
Page 13
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Muraven M, Collins RL, Morsheimer ET, Shiffman S, Paty JA. One too many: Predicting future
alcohol consumption following heavy drinking. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology.
2005; 13(2):127–136. [PubMed: 15943545]
Norusis, MJ. SPSS 12 statistical procedures companion. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall; 2003.
Office of Applied Studies. Results from the 2007 national survey on drug use and health: National
findings. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration; 2008.
(DHHS Publication No. SMA 08–4343, NSDUH Series H-34).
O’Malley PM, Johnston LD. Epidemiology of alcohol and other drug use among American college
students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002; (Suppl. 14):23–39.
Park A, Sher K, Krull J. Risky drinking in college changes as fraternity/sorority affiliation changes: A
person-environment perspective. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2008; 22:219–229.
[PubMed: 18540719]
Pedersen ER, LaBrie JW. Drinking game participation among college students: Gender and ethnic
implications. Addictive Behaviors. 2006; 31:2105–2115. [PubMed: 16600523]
Pedersen ER, LaBrie JW. Partying before the party: Examining “prepartying” behavior among college
students. Journal of American College Health. 2007; 56:237–245. [PubMed: 18089504]
Pedersen ER, LaBrie JW, Kilmer JR. Before you slip into the night you’ll want something to drink:
Exploring the reasons for prepartying behavior among college student drinkers. Issues in Mental
Health Nursing. 2009; 30:1–10.
Read JP, Merrill JE, Bytschkow K. Before the party starts: Risk factors and reasons for “pregaming” in
college students. Journal of American College Health. 2010; 58(5):461–472. [PubMed: 20304758]
Ryback RS. Alcohol amnesia: Observations in seven drinking inpatient alcoholics. Quarterly Journal
of Studies on Alcohol. 1970; 31:616–632. [PubMed: 5458472]
Tabachnick, BG.; Fidell, LS. Using multivariate statistics. 5th ed.. Boston: Allyn and Bacon; 2007.
Warner LA, White HR, Johnson V. Alcohol initiation experiences and family history of alcoholism as
predictors of problem-drinking trajectories. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2007;
68:56–65. [PubMed: 17149518]
Wechsler H, Lee JE, Kuo M, Lee H. College binge drinking in the 1990s: A continuing problem:
Results of the Harvard School of Public Health 1999 College Alcohol Study. Journal of American
College Health. 2000; 48(5):199–210. [PubMed: 10778020]
Wechsler H, Nelson TF, et al. Relationship between level of consumption and harms in assessing drink
cut-points for alcohol research: Commentary on “Many college freshman drink at levels far
beyond the binge threshold” by White et al. Alcohol: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2006;
30(6):922–927.
Wechsler H, Nelson TF. What we have learned from the Harvard School of Public Health College
Alcohol Study: Focusing attention on college student alcohol consumption and the environmental
conditions that promote it. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2008; 69(4):481–490.
[PubMed: 18612562]
White AM. What happened? Alcohol, memory blackouts, and the brain. Alcohol Research & Health.
2003; 27:186–196. [PubMed: 15303630]
White AM, Jamieson-Drake DW, Swartzwelder HS. Prevalence and correlates of alcohol–induced
blackouts among college students: Results of an e–mail survey. Journal of American College
Health. 2002; 51:117–131. [PubMed: 12638993]
White AM, Signer ML, Kraus CL, Swartzwelder HS. Experiential aspects of alcohol–induced
blackouts among college students. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2004; 30(1):
205–224. [PubMed: 15083562]
Zeigler D, Wang C, Yoast R, Dickinson B, McCaffree M, Robinowitz C, et al. The neurocognitive
effects of alcohol on adolescents and college students. Preventive Medicine: An International
Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory. 2005; 40(1):23–32.
LaBrie et al.
Page 14
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
FIGURE 1.
Gender × prepartying days on average drinks during a typical preparty occasion.
LaBrie et al. Page 15
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
LaBrie et al. Page 16
TABLE 1
Chi-square tests between each variable and blacking out on a prepartying night
Blacked out
No Yes
Predictor Level %
n
%
n
Pearson χ
2
Age
Under 21 years 62.8 1,200 63.8 406 0.21
21+ 37.2 710 36.2 230
Gender
6.63
*
Male 39.9 763 45.8 291
Female 60.1 1,147 54.2 345
Race
Caucasian 54.6 1,042 64.6 411
31.65
***
Asian 22.4 427 13.8 88
Hispanic 10.6 203 11.9 76
African American 2.5 47 1.1 7
Other/multiracial 10.0 191 8.5 54
Greek status
82.96
***
No 72.3 1,379 52.8 335
Yes 27.7 528 47.2 300
Family history of alcohol abuse
10.46
***
No 64.4 1,230 57.2 364
Yes 35.6 680 42.8 272
Prepartying days (past 30 days)
364.21
***
1 24.7 472 2.5 16
2–5 55.3 1,056 42.6 271
6–9 13.9 266 29.7 189
10+ 6.1 116 25.2 160
Prepartying alone 1.08
No 95.1 1,816 94.0 598
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
LaBrie et al. Page 17
Blacked out
No Yes
Predictor Level %
n
%
n
Pearson χ
2
Yes 4.9 94 6.0 38
Prepartying with friends/roommates 0.91
No 4.7 90 5.7 36
Yes 95.3 1,820 94.3 600
Prepartying while playing drinking games
89.02
***
No 61.7 1,178 40.3 256
Yes 38.3 732 59.7 380
Prepartying with beer
26.64
***
No 42.8 818 31.9 203
Yes 57.2 1,092 68.1 433
Preparty with shots
43.24
***
No 33.1 632 19.3 123
Yes 66.9 1,278 80.7 513
Prepartying with wine
8.96
**
No 85.1 1,625 80.0 509
Yes 14.9 285 20.0 127
Prepartying with mixed drinks
8.36
**
No 44.8 855 38.2 243
Yes 55.2 1,055 61.8 393
*
p < .05,
**
p < .01,
***
p < .001.
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
LaBrie et al. Page 18
TABLE 2
Binary logistic regression model predicting blacking out on a prepartying night (1 = yes, 0 = no)
Predictor Level
B SE
Wald
OR
a
Age
Under 21 years 1.00
21+ 0.06 0.11 0.30 1.06
Gender
Male 1.00
Female −0.07 0.11 0.41 0.93
Race
Caucasian 1.00
Asian −0.32 0.15 4.83
0.73
*
Hispanic −0.05 0.16 0.10 0.95
African American −0.85 0.44 3.84
0.43
*
Other/multiracial −0.21 0.18 1.27 0.81
Greek status
No 1.00
Yes 0.32 0.11 8.71
1.37
**
Family history of alcohol abuse
No 1.00
Yes 0.21 0.10 4.19
1.24
*
Prepartying days (past 30 days)
1 1.00
2–5 1.85 0.27 48.48
6.36
***
6–9 2.69 0.28 93.26
14.73
***
10+ 3.30 0.29 128.01
27.01
***
Prepartying alone
No 1.00
Yes −0.01 0.22 0.00 0.99
Prepartying with friends/roommates
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
LaBrie et al. Page 19
Predictor Level
B SE
Wald
OR
a
No 1.00
Yes −0.24 0.24 1.06 0.79
Prepartying while playing drinking games
No 1.00
Yes 0.23 0.11 4.11
1.26
*
Prepartying with beer
No 1.00
Yes 0.08 0.12 0.49 1.09
Prepartying with shots
No 1.00
Yes 0.47 0.12 14.61
1.60
***
Prepartying with wine
No 1.00
Yes 0.14 0.14 1.04 1.15
Prepartying with mixed drinks
No 1.00
Yes −0.03 0.11 0.06 0.97
Note. The first level of each predictor variable represents the reference level; SE, standard error.
a
Adjusted odds ratio, after controlling for all other predictors in the model.
*
p < .05,
**
p < .01,
***
p < .001.
Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 February 16.